Henvendelse af 27/5-22 fra Jan Bækgaard Nielsen, Farum

Tilhører sager:

Aktører:


    B 143 - notat - bilag 5 - 27.05.2022

    https://www.ft.dk/samling/20211/beslutningsforslag/b143/bilag/7/2583443.pdf

    Folketinget
    Social og Ældreudvalget
    27. maj 2022
    Ministeren har 25. maj 2022 svaret (doknr. 575192) på henvendelse af 20. maj 2022 (B 143 - bilag 5).
    Af ministerens svar fremgår at ministeren ikke anser, at der er grundlag for at iværksætte en nærmere
    belysning af samværsordninger når et barn pludseligt oplever at blive fraholdt mulighed for samvær med
    den ene forælder, eksempelvis i forbindelse med et samlivsbrud eller et pludseligt initiativ trods
    eksempelvis 7/7 samværsordning med fuldbyrdelsesklausul.
    I ministerens svar refereres der til et citat fra en VIVE rapport, hvor der står:
    ”Analyseresultatet giver imidlertid ikke anledning til at drage konklusioner om, at
    indretningen af samværet er underordnet. Analysen viser netop, at en god relation til moren
    og faren hver især bidrager positivt til barnets trivsel. At udvikle relationer indebærer
    selvsagt, at man har mulighed for at tilbringe tid sammen.”
    Ministeren synes at tillægge sætningen ” Når vi tillige kontrollerer for betydningen af relationelle faktorer,
    særligt om forældrerelationen er konfliktfyldt, opløses den statistiske sammenhæng mellem
    samværsarrangementet og børnetrivsel” særlig vægt.
    Det er problematisk.
    Der er både før og efterfølgende VIVE rapportens tilblivelse, publiceret longitudinelle peer reviewed
    studier, der underbygger at samvær i langt højere grad udgør en beskyttelsesfaktor for barnet, hvis effekt
    mere end kompenserer for samtidig tilstedeværelse af en lang række risikofaktorer herunder ”højkonflikt”
    og ”samarbejdsproblemer”.
    I modsætning til VIVE rapporten har alle disse studier været udsat for en uvildig gennemgang af fagfæller i
    form af et såkaldt peer review og derfor været publiceret i relevante anerkendte internationale journaler,
    hvilket er et udtryk for at data, metode, diskussioner og konklusioner er repræsentative, lødige og fagligt
    up-to-date.
    Ligeledes er der ikke et forskningsmæssigt belæg for at fremhæve ”højkonflikt” og ”samarbejdsproblemer”
    som værende særligt vægtige risikofaktorer, mens der for ”samvær” netop er et forskningsmæssigt belæg
    for at vægte denne beskyttelsesfaktor i langt højere grad end det i dag er tilfældet.
    Derfor synes det at være relevant at underbygge, hvorfor der er grundlag for at iværksætte en nærmere
    belysning af udgangspunkt for indretning af samværsordninger. I det følgende anvendes nyere forskning
    der er nuanceret, fagligt up-to-date og hvis resultater også er overførbare til danske forhold.
    Beklager hvis læsemængden synes stor. Det er dog intet at sammenligne med konsekvenserne for de
    børn, der oplever at miste kontakt med den ene forælder eller ende ud i skæve samværsordninger, med
    en stærkt forøget risiko for at miste kontakten som følge af en uhensigtsmæssig systemisk praksis.
    Offentligt
    B 143 - Bilag 7
    Social- og Ældreudvalget 2021-22
    Spørgsmål til ministeren
    - Anerkender ministeren præmissen at ny applicerbar relevant peer reviewed forskning altid bør
    inddrages af det familieretslige system, når den for barnet mest hensigtsmæssige indretning af
    samværsordninger skal defineres og udmøntes i relevant lovgivning og tilhørende vejledninger,
    samt reflekteres i ansvarlige styrelsers praksis? Hvis ikke, hvorfor?
    - Anerkender ministeren at VIVE og SFI’s rapporter på området ikke har været udsat for uvildig
    fagfællebedømmelse eller gennemgået peer review (bilag 1)?
    - Anerkender ministeren at man i Danmark ikke har udført effektforskning, dvs. longitudinelle studier
    der belyser de langsigtede konsekvenser for børnene af den førte politik på det familieretslige
    område, på samme vis som der heller ikke på anbringelsesområde er evidens for at konkludere, at
    den førte politik reelt er til barnets bedste? Hvis ikke, hvorfor?
    - Anerkender ministeren at den førte politik på det familieretslige område, skal sikre at den
    kortsigtede ”fordel” ikke må indebære tilførsel af problemstillinger på den lange bane, der samlet
    set indebærer at signifikant forøget risiko for at barnet som ung og voksen oplever en væsentligt
    forringet livskvalitet sammenholdt med relevante kontrolgrupper? Hvis ikke, hvorfor?
    - Mener ministeren at det er acceptabelt at operere med nuværende ”faste rammer for
    minimumssamvær”, som også er udgangspunktet når Familieretshuset foretager vejledning, når
    risiko for at barnet over tid mister kontakt til en ikke uegnet forælder er oppe på 50%?
    Baggrund for spørgsmål:
    Den af ministeren omtalte VIVE rapport henviser blandt andet til kildemateriale fra professor Dr. Linda
    Nielsen, men desværre uden at forholde sig til at der er tale om et metastudie, der inddrager 60
    internationale peer reviewed studier fra 15 OECD lande (herunder Sverige), hvis hovedpunkter er:
    1. Forskningens konklusioner gælder for børn i alle aldre.
    2. Uanset forældrenes indbyrdes konfliktniveau og samarbejdsevne konkluderer
    forskningen klart at den ligeværdige deleordning er bedst for barnets udvikling.
    3. Bedre kognitiv udvikling giver et bedre uddannelsesgrundlag
    4. Bedre følelsesmæssig og psykologisk trivsel (angst, depression, selvværd,
    livstilfredshed)
    5. Færre adfærdsproblemer (mindre kriminalitet, destruktiv adfærd, i skolen, mobning;
    stoffer alkohol, rygning)
    6. Bedre fysisk helbred og færre stres relaterede sygdomme
    7. Bedre relation til begge forældre, stedforældre og bedsteforældre
    8. Konfliktniveauet stiger, når en forælder marginaliseres
    9. Børn med deleordninger trives bedre uafhængigt af forældrenes socioøkonomiske
    status
    10. Ved en ligeværdig deleordning bevares barnets ligeværdige relation til begge
    forældre som før skilsmissen
    11. Myten om en primær forælder og en fast base i relation til ”flyttespørgsmålet” er
    ikke relevant fordi ”weekend- og delebørn” flytter lige ofte mellem forældrene. Og
    børnene oplever en normal hverdagsuge med begge forældre.
    12. Børn med deleordningen mister sjældent den vigtige relation til deres
    bedsteforældre.
    Den nyere forskning underkender fuldstændig den praksis man anvender i det danske familieretssystem,
    hvor konflikt eller samarbejdsargumentet i sig selv kan medføre at barnet mister kontakt til en forælder.
    Professor Dr. Nielsen udtaler blandt andet at man reelt påfører barnet et dobbelt traume, ved at barnet
    ikke alene påføres et traume ved forældrenes konflikt, men påføres et endnu større traume ved tab af
    kontakt til en forælder.
    Barnet er, uanset alder, sikret en mere stabil udvikling og trivsel ved bevarelse af kontakten til begge
    forældre uanset konfliktniveau og samarbejdsproblemer. Samvær med særligt uegnede forældre er
    undtaget.
    Efterfølgende er der publiceret andre peer reviewed studier, der dels bekræfter professor Dr. Nielsens
    sammenfattende metastudie, dels tilfører nye indsigter og behovet for og værdien for barnet af ligeværdig
    adgang til begge forældre – på kort og langt sigte.
    Samvær er for barnet en afgørende beskyttelsesfaktor ved "højkonfliktsager"
    Ny peer-reviewed forskning ved professor William Fabricius et al. dokumenterer af ligeligt fordelt
    samværstid udgør en beskyttelsesfaktor, der også på lang sigt mere end kompenserer for risikofaktorerne
    "forældrekonflikt" og ”samarbejdsproblemer” som eksempelvis falske anklager i en sag kan være udtryk
    for.
    Studiet dokumenter at desto mere intensiv forældrekonflikten forekommer, desto vigtigere er det at
    barnets samværstid deles ligeligt, hvilket indebærer at "højkonflikt" / samarbejdsproblemer = 50/50
    ordning – uanset om der er tale om reel konflikt eller konflikten er ensidigt båret (chikane).
    • Forholdet gælder også selvom forældresamarbejdet efter en sags afslutning synes konfliktfyldt eller
    at samværsordningens udformning var truffet ved det familieretslige systems mellemkomst med
    deraf afledt modstand.
    • Studiet viser at øget samværstid med den ene forældre hen imod ligelig fordeling, ikke påvirker
    barnets relation til den anden forælder negativt.
    • Studiets udfald ændres ikke selvom at den forælder, der gives mere samværstid, ville kunne
    betegnes som at have ”en dårlig forældrestil”.
    • Studiets konklusioner var uforandrede, selvom barnet har været vidne til fysisk vold i hjemmet,
    hvor voldsudøvelsen i øvrigt rent kønsmæssigt akkumuleret set var ligeligt fordelt.
    Ved fravær af forældrekonflikt og tilstedeværelse af et konstruktivt samarbejde med barnet i fokus, ses
    samme positive langtidseffekt også i mere skæve samværsordninger (5/9 og 4/10), hvilket er i
    overensstemmelse med hvad man i mange andre peer reviewed studier har fundet.
    Det longitudinelle studie tog udgangspunkt i et repræsentativt udsnit af samværsordninger samt state-of-
    the-art statistiske analysemodeller, hvorved det var muligt at inddrage kritikpunkter andre studier havde
    været udsat for, eller af tidligere studier påpegede usikkerheder med behov for yderligere belysning. Som
    en væsentlig bisætning skal det bemærkes at mange danske analyser/rapporter på området ikke synes at
    have været udsat for et såkaldt peer review, og dermed risikerer at fremstå som uanfægtede synspunkter,
    desværre
    Se bilag 8
    Adskillelse mellem barn og forælder medfører langvarige neurologiske skader ligesom direkte
    sagsinddragelse af det familieretslige system påfører barnet skade grundet emotionelt stress.
    Neurologisk er det ligeledes undersøgt og dokumenteret, hvorledes et barns hjernes udvikling påvirkes
    negativt af adskillelse fra forældre. Desto længere adskillelsen står på desto kraftigere og længerevarende
    påvirkning af barnets evne til at knytte trygge bånd til andre, hvilket er en fundamental forudsætning for at
    kunne udvikle sit læringspotentiale og indgå i stabile følelsesmæssigt givende relationer. Effekterne af
    følelsesmæssigt misbrug og omsorgssvigt er virkelig, virkelig dyb, og svarer fuldstændig til fysisk misbrug
    (vold) eller seksuelt misbrug med hensyn til hjerneeffekter.
    I rapport fra 2016 belyste professor Vittorio Carlo Vezzetti samværsordninger ud fra et
    folkesundhedsmæssigt perspektiv, hvor konsekvenser af adskillelse sammenkædes med eksempelvis
    forhøjet blodtryk, kroniske forøgelse af kortisol hvilket forbindes med adskillige sygdomme i voksenalderen
    og alderdom som psykopatologi (f.eks. depression), diabetes II, fedme og osteoporose. I rapportens
    opsummering lyder det:
    “Finally, the consequences of loss of contact between one of the parents and the child /
    children will result in a heavy burden for future worldwide generations.”
    Hjerneforskning dokumenterer at adskillelse mellem barn og forældre medfører langvarige neurologiske
    skader, afhængig af barnet alder, adskillelsens karakter og omstændigheder, ligesom et barns hjerne kan
    påvirkes negativt verbalt, såvel som af overgreb og misrøgt – skaderne på hjernen er identiske uanset
    årsag.
    Så når man i Danmark inddrager og placerer børn centralt i samværssager, hvor de i en ung alder udsættes
    for et psykologisk pres (vedvarende emotionelt stress), som følge af en eller begge forældres stærke
    interesse eller egne behov kombineret med lange undersøgelsesforløb, da forstærker det familieretslige
    system en risikofaktor, som barnet hverken er udviklet til at forstå eller navigere i – det prøver givetvis efter
    bedste evne, alt efter alder, at imødekomme de forventninger og krav det udsættes for, alt imens det
    pådrager sig skade i forsøget. Amygdala er særligt udsat herfor.
    Amygdala (på dansk mandelkernen) er et lille område i hjernens tindingelap, som blandt
    andet håndterer frygt og forsvarsreaktioner. Den er en del af det limbiske system og har
    forbindelser til hypofysen, binyrerne, lugtesansen og til indtagelse af føde og væske.
    Amygdala kan udløse forskellige viscerale og autonome reaktioner.
    Når Amylgdala udsættes for vedvarende stress kan det føre til:
    ▪ Vedvarende neuronal hypertrofi og symptomer på angst
    ▪ Irreversibel skade
    ▪ Effekt aftager ikke med præfrontal kortikal udvikling
    Amygdala er eksempelvis involveret i posttraumatisk belastningsreaktion …
    Så når man i Danmark vælger skæve samværsordninger eller at suspendere samvær, da øger man risikoen
    for at påføre neurologiske skader på barnet, hvor nogle over tid måske kan heles, men andre er irreversible.
    Derfor kan man passende spørge det familieretslige system, hvilken indsigt man har i dette område,
    hvorledes de forskellige processer er tilrettelagt i forhold hertil, på samme vis som man kan spørge
    hvorledes børnepsykologer, socialrådgivere og jurister inddrager dette i deres arbejde?
    Se bilag 2
    Derfor er det særlig interessant at Center for Familieudvikling d. 9. juni 2022 præsenterer de danske
    erfaringer fra behandlingsprogrammet “Sammen Om Børnene”. Programmet retter sig mod at hjælpe
    familierne ud af de fastlåste konflikter og er baseret på det internationalt anerkendte program “No Kids in
    the Middle”.
    Se bilag 9
    Skæve samværsordninger øger også risikoen for tab af kontakt over tid
    Nyere tysk studie har dokumenteret at risikoen for at barnet mister i opvæksten kontakten med den ene
    forælder, afspejles af samværsordningens indretning:
    ▪ Hvis samværet udgør under 16% er risikoen 50% for at miste kontakt til en forælder
    ▪ Hvis samværet er 50/50 udgør risikoen 10% for at miste kontakt til en forælder
    Hvis man igen kigger på Familieretshusets ”Faste rammer for minimumssamvær” så er der ingen af disse
    der modsvarer et samvær på 16% eller mere.
    Hvis barnet ikke er fyldt 1 år > 1% samvær
    I kan få samvær 1-2 timer hver uge lørdag eller søndag samt 1-2 timer
    en hverdag hver uge efter Familieretshusets nærmere bestemmelse.
    Hvis barnet er 1-3 år > 3% samvær
    I kan få samvær hver anden lørdag eller søndag kl. 10-17 samt en
    hverdag i den modsatte uge kl. 16-18.
    Hvis barnet er 4-6 år > 9% samvær
    I kan få samvær hver anden uge fra lørdag kl. 10 til søndag kl. 17.
    Hvis barnet er fyldt 7 år > 14% samvær
    I kan få samvær hver anden uge fra fredag kl. 17 til søndag kl. 17.
    Selvom ministeren i sit svar anfører at ikke altid opereres med et standardiseret minimumssamvær, da
    synes praksis at være, at det er udgangspunktet i hovedparten af den rådgivning som familieretshuset yder
    jf. bilag 3.
    Man må derfor forholde sig til, om det er acceptabelt at operere med nuværende ”faste rammer for
    minimumssamvær” når risiko for at barnet mister kontakt til en ikke uegnet forælder er oppe på 50%
    Se bilag 5
    Tilknytningsteori vs. dansk praksis
    70 internationale forskere indenfor tilknytningsteori gennemgår i rapporten Attachment goes to court:
    Child protection and custody issues fra januar 2021, vigtigheden af at inddrage børns tilknytning til nære
    relationer, når der træffes afgørelser i det familieretslige system.
    I rapporten fremhæves 3 principper og 8 anbefalinger som i dag ikke i tilstrækkeligt omfang er reflekteret i
    det danske familieretslige systems praksis, hvilket ikke er til barnets bedste.
    Det er evident at børns tilknytning til sine nære relationer vægtes i langt højere grad end det er tilfældet i
    dag, idet beskyttelsesfaktoren over tid i signifikant grad overstiger risikofaktorerne ”højkonflikt” og
    ”samarbejdsproblemer”, som i dekader har været italesat som værende tungtvejende.
    Kildematerialet udgør over 200 kilder.
    Se bilag 6 for principper og anbefalinger
    Familieretshusets samværsguide
    Det følgende er citat fra familieretshusets samværsguide:
    ”Barnets alder som pejlemærker
    0-1 år
    Spædbarnet har i særlig grad brug for forudsigelighed, vante rammer og rutiner. Der er
    desuden fysiske behov som amning, søvn og mad, der skal tages hensyn til. Barnet har brug
    for at have en gennemgående omsorgsperson, som kender barnets signaler. Dette skal være
    den person, som barnet har været mest afhængigt af og er knyttet til.
    Samtidig er det vigtig at kunne have hyppig kontakt til den anden forælder, og at den anden
    forælder inddrages og informeres godt. Dette er vigtigt for at bakke op om relationen til
    begge forældre. Det optimale for barnet er en fast base, og at den anden forælder kan være
    sammen med barnet flere gange om ugen.
    1-3 år
    Når barnet bliver lidt ældre, kan samværet udvides, og efterhånden kan overnatning også
    være en god løsning. Når barnet er under tre år, har det stadig brug for stor stabilitet og
    rytme i sin hverdag, og det er vigtigt at støtte op om dette. Barnet har stadig en begrænset
    tidsforståelse.
    Før man begynder på overnatning, skal barnet være modent nok. Overnatning fungerer
    bedst, hvis der er et godt samarbejde og tæt kontakt mellem forældrene. Det er vigtigt at
    tage hensyn til barnets robusthed og tilknytningen til forældrene.
    3-6 år
    Når barnet er mellem tre og seks år, kan det fungere godt for barnet med mere samvær og
    flere overnatninger. Hvis de fungerer godt for barnet, kan man med tiden gradvist nærme sig
    mere delt tid mellem forældrene. Igen er det afgørende at kigge på barnet, på forholdet til
    begge forældre og på forældrenes samarbejde. Børn i denne alder forstår mere, og man kan
    forberede dem på, hvad der skal ske. De har stadig svært ved at undvære en forælder i en
    hel uge ad gangen og vil derfor have glæde af at se den anden forælder i løbet af ugen.”
    Kilde: https://familieretshuset.dk/media/1576/samvaersguide_170321.pdf
    Som det fremgår af peer reviewed forskning er ovenstående ikke i overensstemmelse med synes
    samværsguiden at afspejle synspunkter og holdninger som var gældende dekader tilbage. Trods adskillige
    forespørgsler til Familieretshuset er det ikke lykkedes at få oplyst hvilket kildemateriale, der ligger til grund
    for ovenstående.
    Professor i udviklingspsykologi Dion Sommer beskriver i kronik fra 2021 vigtigheden for barnet af begge
    forældres aktive tilstedeværelse i dets liv, helt fra spæd. Manden er på samme måde som kvinden
    evolutionært disponeret for at påtage sig faderrollen, på samme vis som kvinden moderrollen – og
    samspillet mellem disse profiterer barnet af på forskellig vis, hvorfor fravær indebærer at begge parter
    (barn – far/mor) ikke stimuleres i tilstrækkelig grad, til skade for alle.
    I kronikken refereres der til observationsstudier som konkluderer:
    1. Involverede fædre er lige så kompetente som mødre til at drage omsorg for
    spædbørn.
    2. Spædbørn knytter sig lige så nært til deres omsorgsfulde far som til deres moder.
    Men ikke til den fraværende far.
    3. Aktive involverede fædre spiller en fundamental rolle for spædbarnets udvikling.
    4. Børn med mere end kun én nær og tryg tilknytning udvikles bedre.
    Se bilag 10
    Det er derfor stærkt problematik at Familieretshusets ”Faste rammer for minimumssamvær” ikke har et
    udgangspunkt, der imødekommer de behov, som barnet reelt har jævnfør nyere forskning, men i stedet
    tager udgangspunkt i skævvredne samværsordninger, der på mange andre områder også øger risici for at
    barnet over tid oplever en væsentligt forringet livskvalitet.
    Folketingets ansvar
    I aftaletekst af 27.3.18 om ét samlet familieretligt system lød det således:
    Regeringen (Venstre, Liberal Alliance og Det Konservative Folkeparti) og Socialdemokratiet,
    Dansk Folkeparti, Enhedslisten, Alternativet, Radikale Venstre og Socialistisk Folkeparti har
    den 27. marts 2018 indgået en stemmeaftale om ét samlet familieretligt system.
    "…. Partierne ønsker at understrege, at de muligheder, der skabes for at understøtte det
    ligeværdige forældreskab, er rettet mod forældre, der kan samarbejde til barnets bedste.
    Partierne ønsker således ikke at skabe situationer, hvor konfliktende forældre i gult spor og
    forældre i rødt spor har systemets opbakning til deleordninger, altså 7/7- eller 6/8-ordning,
    delt bopæl m.v."
    Link: https://www.regeringen.dk/aktuelt/publikationer-og-aftaletekster/aftale-om-
    %C3%A9t-samlet-familieretligt-system/
    Det kan undre, at man fra politisk hold synes at spænde ben for ligeværdige samværsordninger, når nyere
    forskning dokumenterer, at disse på sigt er bedst for barnet jævnfør anerkendt forskning. Samtidig åbner
    det for at spekulere i at skabe ”højkonfliktsager” og samarbejdsproblemer, idet udredning af årsag og
    ansvar ikke indgår i sagsbelysning.
    Det kan også undre, at man fra politisk hold med aftaleteksten nærmest tvinger afgørelser igennem, der
    enten belønner en reelt ikke samarbejde part idet et skifte fra fx. 4/10 til 9/5 synes at være et meget
    markant skifte (hvorfor man måske vælger 5/9 i stedet for som ønsket 7/7 ordning) eller man presses til at
    træffe afgørelser, hvor man fx. går fra 3/10 til 9/5 ordning – ingen af disse tilgange er til barnets bedste.
    Uddybende bemærkninger til den af ministeren omtalte VIVE rapport
    Om den VIVE rapport, der af ministeren refereres til:
    a) Den er udfærdiget af sociologer
    b) Ingen psykologer synes at have medvirket til at kvalificere afsnittet (med start side 246).
    c) Den har ikke været udsat for peer review (se bilag 1)
    d) Den inddrager ganske få af de 60 nyere peer reviewed studier, som indgik i professor Dr.
    Linda Nielsens metastudie, der blev præsenteret i starten af 2018 – dvs. før VIVE
    rapportens publicering.
    I rapporten (s248) står
    ”…. eller seriøse konflikter antages at have betydning for børns udviklingsveje (Felitti et al., 1998).
    For det tredje spiller timingen af samlivsophævelsen en rolle for valg af samværsarrangement:
    Børn, der oplever familiebrud tidligt i livet, får gennemgående mindre omfattende ordninger, end
    børn, der oplever familieopløsning senere i livet (Ottosen, 2001, 2014). Det kan bero på forældres
    og eksperters opfattelser om, at små børn har behov for en stabil og forudsigelig hverdag og derfor
    vil profitere af have en fast base hos den ene forælder – ofte moren.”
    Kommentar:
    Det bemærkes at man i rapporten her vælger at henvise til en kilde fra 1998 samt rapportens
    hovedforfatters egne publiceringer, samt anvender en ikke yderligere underbygget reference til
    ”eksperter”.
    I rapporten (s249) står:
    ”Et mindre omfattende samværsarrangement kan således blive iværksat ud fra en hensigt om at
    beskytte barnet mod yderligere at blive eksponeret for sådanne risici, fordi den ene af forældrene
    ikke magter at udføre sin forældrerolle, eller fordi forældrenes konfliktniveau er for højt til, at en
    deleordning kan fungere (Ottosen, Dahl & Boserup, 2017).”
    Kommentar:
    Man må bemærke at man i rapporten igen henviser til en kilde, som er udarbejdet af
    hovedforfatteren til rapporten med medvirken fra den tidligere underdirektør i Børns Vilkår,
    som ikke har en forskningsmæssig baggrund. Kilden har ikke været udsat for peer review.
    ”…… konfliktniveau er for højt til, at en deleordning kan fungere”
    Kommentar:
    Udsagnet er ligeledes bemærkelsesværdigt, idet det ikke er underbygget på nogen vis.
    Hertil skal det bemærkes at man med udsagnet ikke forholder sig til hvilke muligheder der
    forefindes for at hjælpe med at reducere konfliktniveauet eller omgå dette, ej heller
    inddrager viden om hvorledes man i andre lande gør dette. Eksempler herpå kunne være at
    knytte en til sagen der har til formål at varetage barnets interesser under og efter en
    højkonfliktsag; Barnets Advokat (Sverige), Guardian Ad Litem (US m.fl.), CAFCASS’ samlede
    værktøjskasse (UK)), kommunikationsplatform med moderator tilknyttet, fast track ordning i
    retssystemet (Quebec, Canada), ligesom der i Holland er taget et omfattende initiativ, der er
    nævnt i henvendelse til Social og Ældreudvalget d. 23 maj 2022.
    Specifikt om risikofaktorer:
    En risikofaktor kan bredt defineres som et hvilket som helst forhold hos individet eller i opvækstmiljøet, der
    øger sandsynligheden for en fremtidig negativ psykosocial udvikling, herunder adfærdsproblemer. Man må
    bemærke at risikofaktorer ikke kan anvendes direkte at forklare eller forudsige udfald. Med risikofaktorer
    leder man efter sammenfald i børn og unges reaktioner på bestemte belastninger. Ud fra et statistisk
    standpunkt anvendes et meget stort antal børn og unge til at få viden om, hvilke risikofaktorer de har til
    fælles, hvis de lever under bestemte risikofyldte omstændigheder. Dette er på den ene side en stærk viden,
    fordi den baserer sig på et stort antal af børn og unge. Den viden kan derfor bruges til at gøre den
    professionelle opmærksom på, at lever et barn eller en ung under bestemte risikofyldte
    opvækstbetingelser, så er der risiko for, at deres udvikling påvirkes negativt. 'Afvejning' af risiko- og
    beskyttelsesfaktorer og forholdet mellem dem, findes der ingen opskrift til. Den baserer sig derfor på en
    professionel vurdering, som til enhver tid bør være afdækket og dokumenteret.
    Kilde: https://paedagogik.systime.dk/?id=260
    “Børn i risiko” af Øyvind Kvello, som ofte indgår i diverse rapporters og artiklers kildeliste, er meget
    interessant læsning, idet forfatteren anfører “forskningsresultaterne som viser, hvordan højrisikobørn
    udvikler sig, går ikke alene i forskellig retning, men er også delvist modstridende” (s226).
    Bogens kapitel 5 er i sin helhed interessant, men intet herfra underbygger postulatet at indbyrdes
    forældrekonflikt isoleret set udgør en højrisikofaktor.
    VIVE rapportens forfattere kommenterer rapporten med:
    a) At data ikke rummer information om fædres involvering i børneomsorgen forud for bruddet
    b) At man alene interviewer bopælsforælder for de to yngste årgange, hvilket indebærer at 93% af
    besvarelserne alene er mødres;
    a. Dette indebærer eksempelvis at det alene er den ene part der definerer ”konfliktniveau” /
    samarbejdsproblemer.
    b. Dette indebærer eksempelvis at det alene er den part, der forklarer årsag til at barnet har
    mistet kontakt til samværsforælder.
    c) At der ved interview af de to ældste årgange (11-årige, 15-årige) kan være tilstedeværelse af
    bopælsforælder, men uden af forholde sig til hvorledes dette kan influere på svar.
    Kildematerialet:
    Når man kigger nærmere på VIVE rapportens kildemateriale og sammenholder dette med professor Dr.
    Linda Nielsen's do., ihukommende at begge er publiceret i 2018, ses at:
    VIVE refererer til i alt 40 kilder, professor Dr. Linda Nielsen til 60.
    For 7 af kilderne gælder at de indgår i begge.
    For VIVE's rapports er kildemateriale for 52% vedkommende ældre end 5 år, for professor
    Dr. Linda Nielsen er det tilsvarende tal 32% (trods 50% flere kilder).
    For VIVE's rapport er der for 20% af kildematerialets vedkommende sammenfald mellem
    rapportens hovedforfatter og kildematerialets hovedforfatter.
    For professor Dr. Linda Nielsen metastudie er der for 10% af kildematerialets vedkommende
    sammenfald mellem rapportens hovedforfatter og kildematerialets hovedforfatter.
    For VIVE’s rapport gælder at man inddrager professor Dr. Linda Nielsens forskning samt
    andre fælles kilder, men trods dette konkluderer man modsat rettet. I sig selv bør dette give
    anledning til en kritisk undren fra omgivelserne.
    I rapporten (s246) står:
    ”Nogle studier, bl.a. fra Danmark, har peget på, at skilsmisseforældre, der praktiserer
    deleordninger, er et særligt ressourcestærkt befolkningssegment ….”. Efterfølgende henvises
    der til kildemateriale.
    Kommentar:
    Det eneste kildemateriale, der har et dansk udgangspunkt, er Ottesen & Stage 2012, hvor
    den ene er rapportens hovedforfatter. Denne rapport har heller ikke været udsat for et peer
    review, og er kendetegnet ved mangler/svagheder som blandt andet anført i afsnittet ”VIVE
    rapportens forfattere kommenterer rapporten med:”.
    Der er både før og efterfølgende VIVE rapportens tilblivelse, publiceret nye longitudinelle peer reviewed
    studier, der underbygger at samvær i langt højere grad udgør en beskyttelsesfaktor for barnet, hvis effekt
    mere end kompenserer for samtidig tilstedeværelse af en lang række risikofaktorer; herunder højkonflikt
    og samarbejdsproblemer.
    Myter
    Myter bliver eksempelvis til
    - når der ikke haves et tilstrækkeligt kildekritisk blik
    - når man alene udvælger kilder der i sin helhed understøtter ens synspunkt
    - når man udvælger udsagn eller delelementer fra kilder, der understøtter sit synspunkt, uden at
    medtage hvad der måtte tale imod eller skabe usikkerhed
    - når den underliggende forskning er forudindtaget i sin konstruktion
    - når den underliggende forskning i sine fortolkning af data, anvender antagelser og gisninger med
    henblik på at forklare, forhold som ikke tilstrækkeligt belyst
    - når den underliggende forskning, reelt ikke er forskning - eksempelvis når datamaterialet er så
    spinkelt (omfang) og/eller repræsenterer yderpunkter, der statistisk set anses som særdeles usikre
    - når publicering sker i journaler, hvor man betaler for optagelse og kravene til validering er minimale
    eller ikke eksisterende
    - Når en myte frem til den brede offentlighed, fx. via medier, bøger, præsentationer, indgår i
    undervisningsmateriale etc. da bliver det for alvor en vanskelig størrelse at have med at gøre. Og
    helt galt går det når lovgivning og vejledning hertil reflekterer, at en myte er blevet til en absolut
    sandhed.
    Fra politisk side ses sådanne myter hjulpet på vej pakket ind som "vi skal jo beskytte barnet", "det er til
    barnets bedste" - sætninger eller udtryk der appellerer til følelser, hvorved de nemmere finder accept hos
    modtager, og på det familieretslige område er der ingen der kan være uenige i målet om det at beskyttede
    et barn. Ved at koble sine holdninger til sådanne sætninger, opnår man det retoriske greb, at hvis ens
    holdning bliver udsat for kritik, da er det ensbetydende med at kritikerne ikke vil beskytte barnet.
    I Danmark ses en række myter, der præger praksis på det familieretslige område herunder anbringelser og
    samvær, såvel som har fundet indpas i det omkringliggende økosystem.
    I bilag 11 gennemgås en lang række af myter, også kaldet ”woozles”, der har haft og fortsat har afgørende
    indflydelse på udformning og revision af love og vejledninger på det familieretslige område i Danmark.
    Der er tale om myter, der flittigt udtrykkes holdninger i forbindelse med, anvendes når der udfærdiges
    rapport og analyser, ligesom det indgår i bøger og artikler - ofte også i form af kildemateriale, som der
    henvises til.
    Resume
    Dansk praksis i det familieretslige system strider imod hvad international peer reviewed forskning igennem
    en årrække har dokumenteret er til barnets bedste på lang sigt.
    Aktuel dansk praksis indebærer at man for at undgå to risikofaktor (højkonflikt, samarbejdsproblemer)
    forstærker effekten af en anden risikofaktor (emotionel stress), i stedet for at ibrugtage en
    beskyttelsesfaktor (samvær/tilknytning) der mere end kompenserer for de førstnævnte (højkonflikt,
    samarbejdsproblemer) uden at forstærke risikofaktoren tab af samvær (emotionelt stress).
    Det at man forsøger at beskyttet barnet mod forældrekonflikt eller samarbejdsproblemer, ved at reducere
    eller afbryde samvær, er baseret på myter, som gennemsyrer det familieretslige system og omgivende
    interesseorganisationer.
    Dansk praksis i det familieretslige system synes at medvirke til den faktuelle stigning af børn og unge i der
    kommer i kontakt med psykiatrien, medvirker til at unge ikke gennemfører uddannelse samt påfører
    relationelle og interpersonelle problemstillinger langt ind i voksenlivet = tab af livskvalitet!
    Dansk praksis i det familieretslige system synes præget af en juridisk/politisk tilgang, trods flittig brug af
    ”barnets bedste”. Den manglende inddragelse af psykologiske som helbredsmæssige aspekter og de
    langsigtede konsekvenser heraf er dybt problematisk.
    I aftaletekst af 27.3.2018 om ét samlet familieretligt system har regering sammen med hovedparten af
    Folketingets øvrige partier defineret krav, som er uforenelige med hvad international forskning
    dokumenterer, er til barnets bedste på lang sigt.
    Dansk praksis er således ikke til barnets bedste – og man påfører meget store omkostninger, både på
    individniveau og samfundsøkonomisk som konsekvens heraf.
    I lighed med hvad der ses på anbringelsesområdet, sker der heller ikke en struktureret opfølgning og
    evaluering – et forhold der er påtalt af Rigsrevisionen gennem mere end et årti.
    Børn der udsættes for indgribende afgørelser ved det familieretslige systems mellemkomst, er ikke sikret
    nogen form for efterværnsordning, med mindre at man anser forløb i psykiatrien som en sådan.
    Fem meget bekymrende forhold:
    • Ifølge analyse fra Børns Vilkår estimeres at hvert sjette 11-årige barn ikke har kontakt til den ene
    forælder svarende til ca. 10.300 børn i Danmark.
    • Ifølge årgangsanalyse fra SFI/VIVE estimeres det, at hver tredje 15-årige ikke har kontakt til den ene
    forælder
    • 7 ud af 10 unge savner den forælder, de ikke bor hos jf. undersøgelse foretaget af Børnerådet.
    • Det er børn uden kontakt til den ene af sine forældre, der på lang sigt klarer sig signifikant dårligst.
    • Ifølge Vidensråd For Forebyggelse er der sket en tredobling af tilgangen i psykiatrien af børn/unge i
    med diagnoser i skizofreni-spektret. I rapporten er ”enligt forældreskab” anført som en risikofaktor
    i lighed med hvad international forskning også har dokumenteret, hvilket nuværende praksis i det
    familieretslige system medvirker til at tilføre, herunder som følge af langtrukken sagsbehandling.
    Supplerende faktuel information:
    • Cirka hvert tredje danske barn oplever, at forældrene bliver skilt
    • Cirka 25% af samværsaftaler indgås vha. myndighedsafgørelse (ref. Sundhed.dk)
    • Myndighedsudgangspunktet er, at der ikke kan træffes afgørelse om 7/7 eller 8/6 ordninger hvis
    der er forældrekonflikt eller samarbejdsproblemer, hvilket er indskrevet i Folketingets indstilling til
    etablering af ét samlet familieretligt system i form af Familieretshuset
    • Myndighedsudgangspunktet er ikke 7/7 samvær, hvilket udtrykkes ved at kontaktbevarende
    samvær er aldersdifferentieret samt spænder fra 1-2 timer ugentligt til 2/12 "ordning"
    • 50% at børn i samværsordningen med kontakt 2/12 ordning eller med mindre samvær, mister
    kontakt til samværsforælderen.
    • Ca. 7% af ca. 350.000 skilsmissebørn har bopæl hos deres far
    • Andelen af de unge under 24 år med høj score på stressskalaen er steget til over 31% for de unge
    mænd og over 52% for de unge kvinder. Over en tredjedel af kvinderne har desuden decideret
    dårligt mentalt helbred, mens det gælder for lidt over en femtedel af de unge mænd. For
    befolkningen som helhed er andelen med dårligt mentalt helbred stedet til 17,4% (fra 10% i 2010)
    (ref. Den Nationale Sundhedsprofil 2021)
    Med venlig hilsen
    Jan Bækgaard Nielsen
    Gammelgårdsvej 17
    3520 Farum
    Bilag 1:
    Peer review
    Når forskere har undersøgt et emne, beskriver de, hvad de har gjort og fundet frem til i et manuskript.
    Manuskriptet sender forskerne til et tidsskrift for at få det publiceret, så hele det videnskabelige samfund
    (og i nogle tilfælde offentligheden) kan læse om arbejdet i en videnskabelig artikel.
    Men før det når dertil, skal andre forskere med forstand på emnet gennemgå manuskriptet med kritiske
    briller. De ser blandt andet på:
    - Er statistikken i orden?
    - Kommer forskerne grundigt og præcist rundt om emnet?
    - Er det sproglige helt på plads?
    - Har forskerne husket at tage al relevant forskning med i betragtning (lavet de rette referencer)?
    Fagfællerne læser en anonymiseret version af manuskriptet og sender deres kommentarer tilbage
    redaktøren af tidsskriftet. Kommentarerne kan – i anonymiseret form – blive sendt ud til forfatterne, som
    svarer tilbage.
    Redaktøren tager derefter stilling til, om tidsskriftet vender tommelfingeren op eller ned til at publicere
    studiet.
    Der er stor forskel på de videnskabelige tidsskrifters anseelse (ranking), hvorfor dette også er en parameter
    der skal vægtes, når den videnskabelige artikel vurderes og inddrages som kilde. En del tidsskrifter er
    eksempelvis etableret for at fremme særlige synspunkter, hvorved sandsynligheden for at få optaget en
    artikel er større, hvis dennes fund er i overensstemmelse hermed.
    Artikel fra 2022: https://videnskab.dk/kultur-samfund/hvad-er-peer-review
    Peer review vs. lovgivning
    Helt overordnet synes det problematisk af dansk ”forskning” på området ikke er underlagt peer-review,
    altså en uvildig gennemgang inden publicering af fund og konklusioner i anerkendt journal. Ligeledes synes
    at der i tilrettelæggelse af arbejdet med lovgivning såvel som vejledning hertil, ikke synes at være en
    tilstrækkelig kritisk tilgang eller faglig indsigt, men at der gives politiske særsynspunkter eller
    interesseorganisationer, mulighed for at sætte et uhensigtsmæssigt aftryk og dermed mulighed for praktisk
    udmøntning, hvorved ”til barnets bedste” reelt bliver en tom frase.
    I DR2's Deadline indgik direktøren for Danmarks Frie Forskningsfond Maja Horst i debat, hvor hun i
    underbyggede synspunktet af vigtigheden af der i dansk forskning anvendes peer-review samt man indgår i
    internationalt forskningssamarbejde, idet det medvirker til at løfte kvaliteten (validiteten), giver mulighed
    for international faglig sparring/netværk samt inddragelse af nyere udenlandsk forskning, hvor
    forskningsresultaterne ofte er baseret på et større datagrundlag samt at disse studier har været udsat for
    peer-review.
    I debatten om den frie forskning i Danmark, der pågik i 2021, blev det blandt andet bemærket at Danmark
    udgør et så lille område, at det at rejse konstruktiv kritik af en fagfælles arbejde, kunne være som at træde
    ind i et minefelt – ikke kun af faglige årsager men også med sociale konsekvenser. Et af forslagene for at
    imødegå denne problematik lød på etablering af et nationalt forskningsnævn, men som Maja Horst synes
    inde på, kunne en mulighed være at sikre at kvalitetssikring af dansk forskning skulle ske ved at indarbejde
    forpligtigelser til at indgå i internationalt samarbejde med mål om publicering i internationale journaler.
    Bilag 2:
    Adskillelse – hvad gør det ved hjernen og hvordan kommer det til udtryk
    "Effekterne af følelsesmæssigt misbrug og omsorgssvigt er virkelig, virkelig dyb," siger direktør for
    Developmental Biopsychiatry Research Program på McLean Hospital. "De svarer fuldstændig til fysisk
    misbrug eller seksuelt misbrug med hensyn til hjerneeffekter."
    Nogle ændringer er meget specifikke. For eksempel blev der fundet ændringer i den auditive cortex i
    forbindelse med sprogvanskeligheder hos personer, der blev verbalt misbrugt af deres forældre i
    barndommen. Bredere effekter omfatter en reduceret hippocampus (en struktur involveret i hukommelse
    og indlæring), øget aktivitet i amygdala (et vigtigt center for følelsesregulering) og forstyrrelser i
    forbindelserne mellem disse og andre hjerneregioner.
    Artikel: https://www.spektrum.de/news/borderline-folge-zwischenmenschlicher-traumata-in-der-
    kindheit/1975948
    Præsentation:
    https://drteicher.files.wordpress.com/2015/11/enduring_neurobiological_effects_abuse_reduced.pdf
    (opsummering på side 27-28)
    Ud over sin rolle på McLean Hospital er Dr. Teicher lektor i psykiatri ved Harvard Medical School. Han er
    medlem af flere redaktioner, herunder Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. Dr. Teicher er medlem af
    det videnskabelige rådgivende råd for Juvenile Bipolar Research Foundation og SmartFIT-virksomheden og
    bestyrelsesmedlem i organisationer, herunder Trauma Research Foundation og bestyrelsen for børn, unge
    og familier ved National Academies of Sciences, Engineering og medicin. Han har siddet i eller været
    formand for adskillige revisionsudvalg for National Institutes of Health, udgivet mere end 200 artikler og er
    blevet tildelt 19 amerikanske patenter.
    Om Amygdala: https://da.wikipedia.org/wiki/Amygdala
    NOVA er den mest populære primetime videnskabsserie på amerikansk tv, der afmystificerer de
    videnskabelige og teknologiske koncepter, der former og definerer vores liv, vores planet og vores univers,
    belyser i nedenstående forskning, hvorledes et barns hjernes udvikling påvirkes negativt af adskillelse fra en
    forælder eller begge. Desto længere adskillelsen står på desto kraftigere og længerevarende påvirkning af
    barnets evne til at knytte trygge bånd til andre, hvilket er en fundamental forudsætning for eksempelvis at
    kunne udvikle sit læringspotentiale og indgå i stabile følelsesmæssigt givende relationer.
    Artikel: https://www.pbs.org/wgbh/nova/article/psychological-damage-inflicted-by-parent-
    child-separation-is-deep-long-lasting/
    Video: https://www.pbs.org/wgbh/nova/video/separation-and-childrens-brains/
    Dr. Karleen Lyons-Ruth er professor i psykologi ved Harvard Medical School, klinisk vejleder for Cambridge
    Health Alliance Psychology and Psychiatry træningsprogrammer. Summa Cum Laude kandidat fra Duke
    University og modtog sin PhD i udviklingspsykologi fra Harvard University. Før hun kom til Harvard Medical
    School, gennemførte hun et klinisk praktikophold på McLean Hospital og tjente som post-doc forsker ved
    Department of Child Psychiatry, Boston University School of Medicine.
    Artikel: https://theconversation.com/a-sudden-and-lasting-separation-from-a-parent-can-permanently-
    alter-brain-development-98542
    Jacek Debiec, Assistant Professor / Department of Psychiatry; Assistant Research Professor / Molecular &
    Behavioral Neuroscience Institute, University of Michigan
    2 verdenskrig – bombardementet af London
    Under 2. verdenskrig evakuerede man børn fra London, som med deres forældres accept blev flyttet ud på
    landet for at undgå belastningerne som de regelmæssige bombardementer indebar. Efterfølgende blev der
    udført et studie, hvor man sammenlignende udviklingen for de udflyttede børn, med den langsigtede
    udvikling for tilsvarende børn der var forblevet i London sammen med forældrene under
    bombardementerne. Trods studiets svagheder fandt man tydelige indikationer på at oplevelsen af
    evakuering var forbundet med langvarig psykisk sårbarhed grundet usikker tilknytning (til forældrene som
    følge af adskillelsen). Usikker tilknytning indebærer oftest af man som voksen har svært ved at forstå egne
    følelser og andres følelser, hvilket begrænser evnen til at opretholde og vedligeholde stabile relationer.
    Studie:
    https://www.researchgate.net/publication/5281059_The_evacuation_of_British_children_d
    uring_World_War_II_A_preliminary_investigation_into_the_long-
    term_psychological_effects
    Bilag 3:
    Eksempel fra praksis, beskrevet i forbindelse med udarbejdelse af kandidatspeciale.
    Indhold er forelagt og godkendt af Familieretshuset 2020.
    Situation: Forældre tilbydes 10-15 minutters børnesagkyndig rådgivning (den børnesagkyndige har ikke
    mødt forældrene før) i forbindelse med konfliktmæglingsmøde.
    Børnenes alder: Et barn i vuggestue (1-3 år), et barn i børnehave (4-6 år)
    Citater: Hun (børnesagkyndige) fortæller hvad der lægges vægt på i forhold til børnene, hvis parterne
    ikke kommer frem til en løsning selv under konfliktmæglingen.
    ▪ tilknytningen til barnet fx
    o hvem har haft barsel
    o hvem tilbringer tid med børnene
    o hvem søger de til
    ▪ kontakt oftere, frem for længere tid
    ▪ kun har base ét sted = ingen overnatninger hos samværsforælder
    ▪ samme aftale for begge børn
    når børnene ikke er ældre, kan det ikke anbefales, at man har en 7-7 ordning
    Kildemateriale kan oplyses
    Bilag 4:
    ”Faste rammer for minimumssamvær:
    Hvis barnet ikke er fyldt 1 år
    I kan få samvær 1-2 timer hver uge lørdag eller søndag samt 1-2 timer en hverdag hver uge
    efter Familieretshusets nærmere bestemmelse.
    Hvis barnet er 1-3 år
    I kan få samvær hver anden lørdag eller søndag kl. 10-17 samt en hverdag i den modsatte
    uge kl. 16-18.
    Hvis barnet er 4-6 år
    I kan få samvær hver anden uge fra lørdag kl. 10 til søndag kl. 17.
    Hvis barnet er fyldt 7 år
    I kan få samvær hver anden uge fra fredag kl. 17 til søndag kl. 17. ”
    Kilde: https://familieretshuset.dk/foraeldreansvar/foraeldreansvar/samvaer/samvaer-etablering-foraelder-
    uenig-ikke-kontakt
    Vejledning om forældremyndighed, barnets bopæl og samvær:
    https://www.retsinformation.dk/eli/retsinfo/2020/10064
    Bilag 5
    KiMiss
    Tysk studie etableret i 2012:
    KiMiss-projektet udført af Universität Tübingen undersøger, i hvilket omfang konfliktfyldt
    forældreadskillelse fører til problemer med forældremyndigheden, eller til problemer som forældre-barn
    fremmedgørelse eller misbrug af forældremyndigheden. Forskningsprojektet førte til udviklingen af KiMiss-
    instrumentet, som kvantificerer belastningen på børn, der vokser op under forældres konfliktfyldte
    skilsmisse.
    Barnets tarv = barnets livskvalitet
    Begrebet barns bedste er juridisk set et ubestemt begreb, hvorfor familieretsafgørelser bør være
    underordnet dette udtryk. Den bredt accepterede opfattelse af, at børns trivsel er udefinerbar, overser det
    faktum, at begrebet stammer fra et livskvalitetsbegreb: børns trivsel er en barndomsrelateret livskvalitet,
    og denne kan bestemmes og måles som andre kvalitets-livsmålinger.
    Mål for det relative tab af børnevelfærd gør det muligt at sætte forskellige former for vold i hjemmet i
    relation til hinanden med det formål at give beslutningstagere i familiedomstole og i
    ungdomsmyndigheder, men også forældre og familier, et redskab, der hjælper med de svære og alvorlige
    beslutninger om barnets ve og velvære, metodisk at kunne argumentere korrekt, forståeligt og pålideligt
    Fra 2016/2017 analysen:
    Den samværstid, der er til rådighed for forældre og børn, må ikke falde under 16 % over tid.
    Under en perimeter på 16 % af samværstiden er sandsynligheden for forældre-barn
    fremmedgørelse større end sandsynligheden for ikke-fremmedgørelse, hvilket er en
    uacceptabel risiko.
    'Standardkontaktreglen' (14 dages weekend + halvdelen af ferien) svarer til en andel på cirka
    30 % af tiden og er forbundet med en sandsynlighed for fremmedgørelse på (hele) 30 %.
    Dette udgør stadig en væsentlig risiko, så 'standard' 30% af sæsonhåndteringsreglen skal
    formuleres som en slags minimum.
    Selvom begge forældre tager sig af børn på en fuldstændig afbalanceret måde (f.eks.
    vekslende samværsmodel), kan der stadig forventes en slags grundrisiko på 10 %
    fremmedgørelse. Dette kan tolkes på to måder:
    1) Ud fra en sandsynlighed for fremmedgørelse: Ved en balanceret pasning af
    børn kan det forventes, at forældre-barn fremmedgørelse vil forekomme i
    hvert tiende tilfælde.
    2) Med hensyn til en grad af fremmedgørelse: Under afbalanceret omsorg for
    børn rapporterer forældrene en gennemsnitlig fremmedgørelsesgrad på 10 %.
    Link: https://www.kimiss.uni-tuebingen.de/index.html
    Bilag 6
    Attachment goes to court: Child protection and custody issues
    Principper:
    Princip 1: Et barn har brug for at opleve sikre havne fra særlige, velkendte og ikke-
    misbrugende omsorgspersoner
    .
    To overvejelser er centrale:
    a) Begrænset kontakt med en omsorgsperson gør det sværere for et barn at
    danne, styrke og vedligeholde forventninger til denne omsorgspersons
    tilgængelighed i tider med behov.
    b) Næsten al ikke-misbrugende og ikke-forsømmende familiebaseret pleje vil
    sandsynligvis være bedre end institutionspleje (s. 25).
    Princip 2: Sikker, kontinuerlig, "god nok" omsorg er i barnets bedste interesse, og
    omsorgspersoner bør hjælpes til at yde den.
    Princip 3: Oprethold et barns eksisterende sikre havn, hvis de ikke udgør en trussel.
    En beslutning om at opretholde et barns eksisterende sikre havn definerer ikke en plan
    for fordeling af tid i samværsordninger. Tiden skal være tilstrækkelig til, at
    tilknytningsrelationer kan udvikles og vedligeholdes (s. 28).
    Dette princip kan også gælde for plejefamilier, hvor relationer til biologiske forældre
    kan opretholdes under pleje. På samme måde kan relationer til plejefamilier
    opretholdes efter plejefamilien (s. 29).
    Otte specifikke råd til det familieretslige system:
    1. Sæt ikke lighedstegn mellem tilknytningskvalitet og omsorgspersonens følsomhed.
    2. Sæt ikke lighedstegn mellem tilknytningskvalitet og relationskvalitet.
    3. Fortolk ikke børns engangsadfærd som pålideligt indikator for tilknytningskvalitet.
    4. The Tender Years Doctrine er forkert.
    5. Overnatning med en anden forælder er ikke i sig selv skadelig for børn.
    6. At adressere og reducere konflikter er nøglen.
    7. Sikre, at fagfolk i familieretten er tilstrækkeligt uddannet i tilknytningsvurdering.
    8. inddrag beviser direkte fra eksperter, ikke via repræsenterende parter.
    Link: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/14616734.2020.1840762
    Bilag 7
    Metastudie - børn i ligeværdige samværsordninger profiterer på sigt bedst - også trods forældrekonflikt -
    publiceret 2018
    Professor Dr. Linda Nielsens metastudie er baseret på peer reviewed studier fra 15 OECD lande. Hun
    rådgiver og underviser lovgivere, dommere, advokater og andre relevante fagfolk i relation til den
    internationale forskning.
    Metastudiet kan rekvireres ved henvendelse til forfatteren.
    I denne præsentation gennemgås hendes forskningsresultater på området:
    Link: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=t04rGz1haEs
    Kontakt:
    Link: https://education.wfu.edu/about-the-department/faculty-and-staff-profiles/dr-linda-
    nielsen/
    Bilag 8
    Longitudinelt studie - samvær som beskyttelsesfaktor
    - publiceres 2022
    Studiet er et nationalt studie, forankret i organ under U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
    Der er tale om et banebrydende videnskabeligt studie der dokumenter at en ligeværdig samværsfordeling
    udgør en beskyttelsesfaktor, der mere end opvejer den risikofaktor, som oplevelse af indbyrdes
    forældrekonflikt, historisk set er opfattet som.
    Professor William Fabricius, der har medvirket i delstatslige og nationale projekter, herunder indretning af
    lovgivning for det familieretslige system, gennemgår dele af studiet i denne præsentation:
    Link: https://youtu.be/6QryEkEcnyw
    Kilder:
    Fabricius, W.V., Suh G.W., Tyrell, F.A., Sokol, K., Stevenson, M.M., 2022 (in preparation).
    Protecting the children of divorce: Shared Parenting Time, Emotional Security, and Well-
    Being.
    Stevenson, M.M., Fabricius, W.V., Braver, S.L. and Cookston, J.T., 2018. Associations between
    parental relocation following separation in childhood and maladjustment in adolescence and
    young adulthood. Psychology, public policy, and law, 24(3), p.365.
    Fabricius, W.V. and Suh, G.W., 2017. Should infants and toddlers have frequent overnight
    parenting time with fathers? The policy debate and new data. Psychology, Public Policy, and
    Law, 23(1), p.68.
    Diaz, P., Sokol, K.R., Fabricius, W.V. & Braver, S.L. 2012, "Parenting Time, Parent Conflict,
    Parent–Child Relationships, and Children’s Physical Health" in Parenting Plan Evaluations
    Oxford University Press, .
    Fabricius, W.V. and Luecken, L.J., 2007. Postdivorce living arrangements, parent conflict, and
    long-term physical health correlates for children of divorce. Journal of family psychology,
    21(2), p.195.
    Fabricius, W.V. and Braver, S.L., 2006. Relocation, parent conflict, and domestic violence:
    Independent risk factors for children of divorce. Journal of Child Custody, 3(3-4), pp.7-27.
    Braver, S.L., Ellman, I.M. and Fabricius, W.V., 2003. Relocation of children after divorce and
    children's best interests: New evidence and legal considerations. Journal of Family
    Psychology, 17(2), p.206.
    Andre præsentationer ved professor William Fabricius, som også berører emnet:
    Link: https://youtu.be/JfqxbPa6OT4 Link: https://youtu.be/5zBvezI5XkI
    Link: https://youtu.be/juWdnsSMQjU Link: https://youtu.be/t95jqvo2f7M
    Bilag 9:
    Center for Familieudvikling
    En betragtelig del af de danske skilsmissefamilier præges af et højt og vedvarende konfliktniveau. De
    fastlåste konfliktmønstre ødelægger forældresamarbejdet og skaber et utrygt og usundt opvækstmiljø for
    de børn, der vokser op midt i konflikten.
    Samtidig er skilsmissefamilier præget af højkonflikt notorisk svære at hjælpe. I Danmark bruger vi enorme
    ressourcer på juridiske afgørelser i Familieretten, og vores kommunale hjælpesystemer er ofte på
    overarbejde i forsøget på at afbøde den mistrivsel, børn fanget i højkonflikte skilsmisser udviser. Vi savner
    gode værktøjer til at hjælpe forældrene med at bryde selve de konfliktmønstre, der ligger til grund for
    problemerne.
    Behandlingsprogrammet “Sammen Om Børnene”
    I perioden 2019-2022 har Center for Familieudvikling med støtte fra Socialstyrelsen udviklet
    behandlingsprogrammet “Sammen Om Børnene”. Programmet retter sig mod at hjælpe familierne ud af de
    fastlåste konflikter og er baseret på det internationalt anerkendte program “No Kids in the Middle” som
    oprindeligt er hollandsk.
    Erfaringer præsenteres på konference d. 9. juni 2022.
    Link: https://www.familieudvikling.dk/det-goer-vi/konference-hoejkonflikte-skilsmisser/
    Bilag 10
    Professor i udviklingspsykologi: Mit syn på, hvad fædre og deres børn kan sammen, er blevet
    revolutioneret
    Den nære aktive fader spiller en helt unik rolle for børns tilknytning og udvikling – hvis han ellers må eller
    vil. Nyfødte og spædbørn er ikke forprogrammeret til kun at knytte sig til en mor.
    KRONIKEN 27. OKT. 2021 KL. 15.21
    DION SOMMER
    Dion Sommer er professor i udviklingspsykologi ved Aarhus Universitet. Han har udgivet adskillige bøger om
    barndom og børns udvikling og læring.
    Jeg er far til to voksne døtre og mindes begge deres fødsler, som var det i går. Også de massevis af nætter
    med sutteflasker, gående rundt, så de kunne bøvse, og den slidte mor kunne sove. Aktiv deltagelse i deres
    opvækst og liv samt dyb glæde ved stadigvæk at være far for dem er berigende.
    Så det har været både lærerigt og udfordrende at komme nogenlunde helskindet igennem ’praksisprøven’.
    Samtidig har jeg som forsker arbejdet på at bestå ’teoriprøven’; dvs. at kunne den enestående forskning om
    fædre og deres spædbørn udenad. Den har revolutioneret mit syn på, hvad fædre og deres børn kan
    sammen. Den nære aktive fader spiller nemlig en helt unik rolle for børns tilknytning og udvikling – hvis han
    ellers må eller vil.
    Set med et nøgternt forskerblik verserer der for tiden mange myter og stærke følelser om moderskab og
    faderskab. Når det kommer til forlænget øremærket barselsorlov til fædre tyr både kvinder og mænd til
    biologiske kønsdikotomier.
    Sådanne myter har det desværre med at undergrave vores evne til at argumentere for den rigtige
    beslutning om barsel.
    Mit svar er at gå til den fordomsfri far-barn-forskning og vise, hvad den har fundet. For der mangler ikke
    forskning om fædre og deres omsorgsevner. Heller ikke om nyfødte og spædbørn. De er f.eks. ikke
    forprogrammeret til kun at knytte sig til en mor.
    Men forskningen er desværre relativt ukendt herhjemme. Det gælder også i mit fag, udviklingspsykologien,
    hvor der især er problemer i det, der kaldes tilknytningsteorien. Dvs. med hvem og hvornår barnets dybe,
    nære, trygge og tillidsfulde følelsesmæssige bånd opbygges.
    Her er tilknytningsteoriens ’fader’ John Bowlbys teori særdeles indflydelsesrig, og den er fast pensum på en
    række professionsuddannelser.
    Bowlbys idé om den unikke moder resonerer endvidere med nogle dybfølte forestillinger i tiden: det
    ’monotropiske’ tilknytningssyn: Den biologiske moder bør – når alt skrælles væk – være barnets første og
    eneste ultimative tilknytningsfigur. Far eller andre, f.eks. bedsteforældre eller plejeforældre, må ikke
    komme i vejen som tilknytningsfigurer.
    Mandens rolle er derimod at være den gode, opmærksomme partner, der emotionelt og praktisk støtter
    den babyopslugte moder. Men far må aldrig bryde ind i og forstyrre det sårbare mor-barn-bånd.
    Et centralt argument mod en tidlig nær fader-barn-tilknytning findes i John Bowlbys evolutionstese om
    ’hormonel sensitivering’: Når det kommer til succesrig reproduktion af slægten, har evolutionen selekteret
    således, at homo sapiens’ mænd og kvinder er blevet biologisk og psykologisk grundlæggende forskellige.
    Gennem hormonelle forandringer i graviditeten bliver kvinden i særlig grad tunet ind på sit kommende
    moderskab.
    En mand har ikke de samme hormonelle niveauer. Derfor bliver hans omsorgsinstinkt langt mindre stærkt
    og mindre konsekvent udløst af barnet end den biologiske moders instinkt.
    Mænd har nemlig to afgørende biologiske og evolutionære handikap: De har ikke været igennem en
    graviditet, hvor kvindekroppens hormonologi sensitiverer til det kommende barn. De kan ikke amme og
    dermed heller ikke producere den børnesensitiverende laktose.
    Løbet er kørt for faderen, da hormonel sensitivering ikke kan ske, når barnet er født – altså ifølge det
    monotropiske tilknytningssyn. Købes det argument, står vi med en både naturgiven kønsrolledikotomi og
    en modercentrisme.
    Det handler om en urokkelig evolutionær arv: Dybt nede i enhver moderne kvinde findes den oprindelige
    ’stammoder’. Faderen er derimod rangeret ud som tidlig og nær tilknytningsfigur. Hvorfor så give længere
    barsel til en sådan inferiør mand? Lad dog familien vælge selv, og send ham på arbejde.
    Men at kun moderen hormonelt bliver tunet tidligt ind på sit barn, er nu grundigt blevet skudt ned af en
    række undersøgelser. Revolutionerende biopsykologisk forskning i fædres hormonologi har helt
    undermineret det modercentriske evolutionsargument.
    Evolutionen har faktisk positivt selekteret for en ’homo pater’; dvs. en potentielt omsorgskompetent nær
    faderfigur. I aktive involverede fædre stiger nemlig laktose-, prolaktin-, oxytocin- og østrogen-niveauerne
    markant. De involverede processer herimellem er på forskellige måder relateret til både omsorgsevne og
    sensitivitet over for nyfødte og spædbørn. Niveauerne når endog samme højder i fædre som i biologiske
    mødre.
    Sagt i al korthed handler det om, at evolutionen tilsyneladende ikke kun har sensitiveret kvinder hormonelt
    til moderskabet, men også mænd til faderskabet.
    Det er viden, der kan virke temmelig forstyrrende på kønsrolleopfattelsen af ’rigtig’ mandighed og
    maskulinitet. Men er det kun den gravide og ammende kvinde, der har ret til at have sine
    hormonforandringer? Og bliver nogle mænd ligefrem angstprovokeret af, at de også får dem som fædre?
    Evolutionen er dog helt ignorant over for følelsesbelagte argumenter.
    At kun moderen hormonelt bliver tunet tidligt ind på sit barn, er nu grundigt blevet skudt ned af en række
    undersøgelser
    Homo sapiens’ reproduktive succes gennem dens omskiftelige og farefulde forhistorie stiger mere end 60
    procent, når barnet har mere en kun én kvalificeret forælder.
    Forskning har nu også påvist et markant højere prolaktinniveau i fædre sammenlignet med i ikke-fædre.
    Prolaktin kaldes populært omsorgshormonet, da det sensitiverer opmærksomheden og fintuner samspillet
    med babyer.
    Bemærk ordet ’sensitiverer’. Der står ikke: ’determinerer’ fædres opmærksomhed eller adfærd. Mennesket
    er på ingen måde en robot, styret af sine hormoner. Men de kan, ifølge den biopsykologiske forskning,
    påvirke, hvad mænd er opmærksomme på i deres nære personomgivelser. Desuden stiger mænds
    prolaktinniveau efter barnets fødsel, og det er allerhøjest, når barnet er helt spædt.
    Hos de fædre, som er mest kompetente til at dæmpe barnets gråd, ses det højeste prolaktinniveau. Et
    forhøjet prolaktin- og oxytocinniveau sensitiverer manden som far og gør ham særlig opmærksom på sit
    barns signaler. Det er netop et vigtigt kriterium for tilknytningsfigurens omsorgsevne.
    Man har også sammenlignet moderens og faderens oxytocinniveauer. Måske er hendes trods alt højere?
    Nej, de to forældres oxytocinniveauer er helt ens. Selv om oxytocinudskillelse finder sted ved fødsel og
    amning, er der ingen forskel på moderens og faderens oxytocinkoncentrationer. Det underminerer igen den
    montropiske evolutionært baserede idé, at kun en moder er spædbarnets første og eneste omsorgskyndige
    tilknytningsfigur.
    Ifølge nyere antropologisk forskning er det monotropiske standpunkt endog en myte, helt uden historisk
    opbakning: ’Alloparenting’ - dvs. flere omsorgsfigurers pasning af barnet – har faktisk været mest udbredt
    gennem menneskehedens historie.
    Det eksklusive moder-barn-forhold udgør således en både evolutionær og historisk undtagelse
    Det eksklusive moder-barn-forhold udgør således en både evolutionær og historisk undtagelse. Der er f.eks.
    også kastet et kritisk blik på den evolutionsidé, at den ældre kvinde uden for den reproduktive alder er
    funktionelt unyttig og dermed en cost-benefit-beregnet udgift i forhistoriske epoker.
    Men arkæologien har nu indrulleret bedsteforældre som en medforklaring på homo sapiens’ store
    reproduktive succes.
    Det samme gælder fædre i den klassiske evolutionslære om jæger-samler-kulturer. Dvs. i den forhistoriske
    epoke, hvor vores genetiske sammensætning blev grundlagt.
    Men nu er periodens kvinde- og manderoller blevet genfortolket: ’Manden-som-kun-jæger’ er kun delvist
    korrekt.
    Jagten på vildtet var så usikker, at kvinden som den ’sikre samler’ i høj grad bidrog til overlevelsen. Jægeren
    befandt sig derfor ofte på bopladsen i umiddelbar nærhed af børnene. I dag finder vi et jæger-samler-
    samfund, der ligner:
    Enestående observationer af aka-jæger-samler-folket i Afrika viser, at faderen er særdeles involveret i den
    primære omsorg. Når moderen ofte er ude at samle, er spædbarnet mindre end en armslængde fra
    faderen. Med andre ord har en typisk aka-far årelang ’orlov’ sammen med sine børn, helt fra de er ganske
    små.
    For aka-folket vil danske fædres langt mere sporadiske kontakt med deres børn være helt uforståelig – ja,
    ligefrem unaturlig.
    Aka-fædrene sidder oftest med barnet tæt på kroppen og monitorerer dets behov – dæmper eksempelvis
    gråd og uro ved at synge, vugge og holde om det. Når de bliver større, bliver kontakten mere legende og
    udforskende.
    En sådan faderinvolvering kan ikke undgå at få positive konsekvenser for det nære følelsesmæssige bånd
    mellem far og barn. Aka-børnene med deres i vores øjne ’unormalt’ tætte og langvarige faderkontakt
    vokser op og bliver fuldstændig velfungerende voksne i landsbyen. Den hormonale sensitivering finder da
    også sted i den maskuline og veltrænede aka-jæger, som den gør i den danske håndværker, som er aktiv
    far.
    Så der er rigelig forskning, som underminerer ideen om, at hormonel sensitivering kun sker i en moder, og
    at hun er den eneste ene for spædbarnet.
    Homo sapiens’ evolution har derfor ikke kun udviklet en ’stammoder’, men også en ’homo pater’. Men der
    er mere interessant viden om evolutionens skabelse af den nære, kompetente far: F.eks. falder aktive
    fædres testosteronniveau, hvilket dæmper konkurrencelyst, utålmodighed og irritation.
    Testosteronniveauet er særlig højt, når manden er involveret i den ’kurtiserende’ partnerorienterede fase,
    men falder, når han er sammen med sit spæde barn. Faldet gør dog ikke manden mere feminin.
    Testosteronniveauet fluktuerer nemlig, som på et termometer: Er far til konkurrencesport eller i
    træningscenteret, er niveauet højt. Når han så er den nære far hjemme, falder det igen.
    Far-barn-tilknytningen er unik, da den ikke kun skabes i den stille omsorg, men også gennem fysisk robust
    legeaktivitet
    Evolutionen har altså sørget for, at mænd, der selv indleder fysisk kontakt med deres nyfødte barn, straks
    påvirker deres indre hormonelle termometer.
    Med andre ord kan fædre via bevidst målrettet handling aktivt skabe deres egen hormonologi. Dermed
    sensitiverer de sig selv som en begyndende kompetent fader. Hvis de altså vil.
    Resultaterne viser nemlig, at de endokrine hormonstigninger, som sensitiverer fædre til kompetent
    omsorg, ikke som hos moderen starter indefra. Det skabes kun ’by doing’, dvs. i aktiviteten med et barn.
    Det er således ikke noget, man kan vente på bare sker.
    Empiri viser endvidere: Jo tidligere et far-barn-tilknytningsforhold opbygges, og jo længere det varer, desto
    bedre tilknytningssikkerhed opnår barnet. Og desto bedre livslangt forhold får de til hinanden.
    Men mor rangeres ikke ud på sidesporet: Selv ganske spæde børn er i stand til at knytte sig nært til flere,
    der vil drage kvalificeret omsorg for dem. Flerpersonstilknytninger er i dag faktuelt det typiske mønster for
    dagens spædbørn. Ellers ville den udbredte såkaldte alloparenting jo heller ikke have været mulig i
    menneskehedens farefulde historie.
    Fædre kan således – sammen med mødre – blive barnets nære, tillidsfulde og sikre tilknytningsbase.
    Forskningen viser imidlertid, at fædres samværsform med deres børn er ret speciel.
    Fædres hormonelle ændringer med fald i testosteron og stigning i prolaktin og oxytocin ses nemlig ikke kun
    i sikker-base-rolige omsorgssituationer. En undersøgelse foretaget ved barnets seksmåneders alder påviser
    højere niveauer af faderlig oxytocin i legende, stimulerende adfærd (rough and tumble play), som omfatter
    både aktiv berøring og leg med legetøj.
    Det åbner for en ny antagelse: Far-barn-tilknytningen er unik, da den ikke kun skabes i den stille omsorg,
    men også gennem fysisk robust legeaktivitet. Den støttes af, at fædrene med de højeste prolaktin- og
    oxytocin-baselines er allermest involveret i legende udforskende samvær med barnet.
    Kan trangen til denne lystprægede legende aktivitet hos mænd/fædre forklares (delvist) ved et
    belønningssystem i hjernen?
    Ja, oxytocin stimulerer nemlig udskillelsen af belønningsstoffet dopamin og fremkalder en slags
    ’lykketilstand’.
    Der udløses således mest dopamin i faderen, når han indgår i oxytocinstimulerende leg og aktivitet med
    barnet. Men hvad sker der i barnet?
    Man må formode, at barnets lyst til samvær med sin far også stimuleres, idet dets dopaminerge
    belønningssystem ligeledes aktiveres. Lad mig give et eksempel på denne type aktivitet: I den vilde leg
    kastes barnet op i luften for dernæst at suse ned i fars favn under larm og latter i genforeningen. Barnets
    glade hvin, opspærrede øjne og basken med arme og ben opfordrer til gentagelse, hvilket aktiverer
    faderens belønningssystem.
    Den særlige tilknytning mellem far og barn opstår således i en positiv synkroni, som påvirker og påvirkes af
    både fars og barns hormonelle belønningssystemer.
    At ’homo pater’, evolutionens nære kompetente fader, også eksisterer i dag, er ligeledes påvist i en række
    observationsstudier.
    De konkluderer: 1) Involverede fædre er lige så kompetente som mødre til at drage omsorg for spædbørn.
    2) Spædbørn knytter sig lige så nært til deres omsorgsfulde far som til deres moder. Men ikke til den
    fraværende far. 3) Aktive involverede fædre spiller en fundamental rolle for spædbarnets udvikling. 4) Børn
    med mere end kun én nær og tryg tilknytning udvikles bedre.
    Vi har nu set, hvordan nøgtern empirisk evidens har forkastet centrale, afgørende argumenter bag en
    biologisk/evolutionær kønsdikotomi om mødre og fædre. Er der noget bedre argument for tidlig og
    forlænget barselsorlov til fædre? Vel at mærke en orlov, der ikke skal tages fra moderen.
    Kilde: Kronik, Politiken 27. januar 2021
    Kontakt, Dion Sommer: https://pure.au.dk/portal/da/persons/dion-sommer(73370902-c0fe-4068-
    a330-7f24da7bfaa7).html
    Tilknyttet Aarhus Universitet, Psykologisk Institut
    Særlige interesseområder:
    Ny international børneforskning med henblik på konsekvenser for traditionelle børnepsykologiske teorier.
    Fødsel- og familiedannelse.
    Familie- og daginstitutionsforskning, dansk såvel som internationalt. Legeteorier. Senmoderne faderskab.
    Socialpolitik inden for børneområdet: Danmark, Norden og EU. Anvendte aspekter af børneforskning.
    Bilag 11
    Myter (woozles) i og omkring det familieretslige system
    A:
    “Stemming the Tide of Misinformation: International Consensus on Shared Parenting and Overnighting”
    Af Richard A. Warshak, Ph.D
    Gennemgang og analyser af mere end fire årtiers forskning udmøntede sig i 2016 i en peer-reviewet
    konsensusrapport om samværsordninger for mindre børn. Som forventet udløste rapporten en bølge af
    misinformation, der truede med at genoplive myter om børns udvikling og forankre dem i professionel
    praksis og familieret. Listen over de 110 forskere, der har gennemgået og godkendt konsensusrapporten og
    deres professionelle erfaringer afspejler den udbredte accept af konsensusrapportens konklusioner, der
    favoriserer delt forældreskab og overnatning for små børn under normale omstændigheder. Seks år efter
    dens offentliggørelse forbliver konklusionerne og anbefalingerne fra Warshak-konsensusrapporten støttet
    af videnskaben."
    Link: https://www.ft.dk/samling/20151/almdel/SOU/bilag/273/1628385.pdf
    Tilgået Social- og Indenrigsudvalget 2015-16
    SOU Alm.del Bilag 273
    B:
    “Woozles: Their Role in Custody Law Reform, Parenting Plans, and Family Court”
    Analyse ved professor Dr. Linda Nielsen, Wake Forest University (Professor, Educational Psychology)
    Profil: https://education.wfu.edu/about-the-department/faculty-and-staff-profiles/dr-linda-nielsen/
    Liste over publikationer: https://www.researchgate.net/scientific-contributions/Linda-Nielsen-2002738038
    Materiale kan rekvireres ved direkte henvendelse til professor Dr. Linda Nielsen
    Analysen er vist i sin helhed, for derigennem at eksemplificere hvorledes myter bliver til og de negative
    langtidseffekter de kan afstedkomme.
    Analyse i sin helhed:
    (A ‘woozle is similar to a ‘factoid’ in that only at its hearts does the grain of truth lie – but as
    we shall see from the explanation displayed below, it is also similar to a ‘red herring’,
    however, the consequences of woozles are arguably even graver).
    “Woozles: Their Role in Custody Law Reform, Parenting Plans, and Family Court”
    by Prof. Linda Nielsen, Wake Forest University, NC, USA
    Dr. Linda Nielsen is Professor of Adolescent & Educational Psychology and author of “Father-
    Daughter Relationships: Contemporary Research & Issues” (Routledge, 2012). She can be
    contacted at Wake Forest University, North Carolina. www.wfu.edu/~nielsen and emailed
    at: Nielsen@wfu.edu
    Online First Publication, February 10, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/law0000004
    In the international debates on custody law reform and in individual custody decisions in
    families and in courts worldwide, social science research is often misused and abused. In this
    article I describe the process by which data can become distorted in ways that steer
    policymakers, family court personnel, and parents off course in regard to child custody
    decisions. I illustrate this process with a recent study that has garnered international
    attention and influence.
    Keywords: shared custody, joint custody, parenting plans, overnighting (sleepovers).
    One of the most complex and controversial issues confronting the family court system,
    divorcing parents, and legislative bodies is: When parents separate, what is best for children
    in terms of how much time they should live with each parent? When do children benefit
    most from living primarily with one parent or from living with two parents more equally?
    Under what circumstances is spending frequent overnight time in their father’s care not
    beneficial for children, especially for infants and preschoolers? Given the wide range of
    circumstances that can affect family dynamics and children’s well-being, how can it be
    determined which overnight parenting plans are best for children? At the legislative level the
    question becomes: How should custody laws be revised to better meet the needs of children
    in contemporary families?
    The debates over questions such as these become especially controversial and heated when
    the children involved are infants or children under the age of 5 years. Decisions on these
    policy issues can benefit from social science research, if the research is methodologically
    sound and properly applied.
    The purpose of this article is not to advocate for any particular parenting plan or to present
    the research on shared parenting custody controversies. Rather it is to describe the way in
    which social science data can be used to steer policymakers, family court personnel, and
    parents off course in regard to parenting plans and custody law reform—and to illustrate this
    process with a recent study that has garnered international attention in regard to parenting
    plans for infants and other children 4-years-old and younger.
    The aim is to answer two questions: How can social science data be used to “woozle” us into
    believing things that are not true or that are only partially true? Is there any study that
    illustrates the process and the impact of woozling with regard to parenting plans or custody
    law reform for infants and pre-schoolers ?
    What Is a Woozle?
    Nearly 30 years ago, Richard Gelles (1980) popularized the concept of the “woozle effect.” A
    sociologist whose area of expertise was the research on domestic violence, Gelles (1980) was
    concerned about how this research was frequently misrepresented and misused by advocacy
    groups for their own political purposes. In particular, he was troubled because only those
    studies that supported a particular advocacy position – many of which were seriously flawed
    – were being presented as “the” research evidence, while those studies refuting the position
    were being ignored. As a consequence, many false beliefs about domestic violence were
    perpetuated – beliefs Gelles (1980) referred to as woozles.
    Gelles, along with Beverly Houghton who originally coined the term woozle effect
    (Houghton, 1979), compared these distortions and manipulations of the data to the
    children’s story, Winnie the Pooh (Milne, 1926). In the story Winnie the Pooh dupes himself
    and his friends into believing that they are being followed by a scary beast—a beast he calls a
    woozle. Although they never see the woozle, they convince themselves it exists because they
    see its footprints next to theirs as they walk in circles around a tree. The footprints are, of
    course, their own. But Pooh and his friends are confident that they are onto something really
    big.
    In fact, their foolhardy actions are based on faulty “data—a woozle (Gelles, 1980). More
    recently, Gelles (2007) described the process of woozling as “the use, abuse and misuse” of
    social science research. As defined by Gelles (1980), a woozle is a belief or a claim that is not
    supported—or is only partially or tentatively supported – by the empirical evidence. But
    because the claim has been repeatedly cited and presented in misleading ways, the public
    and policymakers come to believe it. As a result, data that are not accurate or that are only
    partially accurate come to be accepted as the “scientific evidence” on that particular topic.
    Put differently, a woozle is a definitive statement based on data that are very limited,
    flawed, ambiguous, or erroneous. Through a number of different “woozling” techniques,
    these flawed, scanty, or inaccurate data become magnified and widely disseminated,
    overshadowing data that would challenge it. Certain aspects of the woozle might be partially
    true in that some findings in a few studies can be interpreted in ways that lend some support
    to portions of the woozle. That is, there might be a small grain of truth buried in a bushel of
    untruths – which is one reason why woozles are so hard to challenge.
    Often a woozle also has an inherent appeal to people’s prevailing beliefs or to their personal
    feelings about a particular topic, which also makes it difficult to dismantle the woozle.
    Eventually the processes that have promoted the woozle are successful enough that the
    flawed, limited, or exaggerated data come to have an impact on public opinion and public
    policy.
    As Winnie the Pooh and his friends who are led astray by their own footprints, we are led
    astray by the questionable, limited, or contradictory evidence underlying the woozle.
    Describing the woozling process in the field of physics in his book, Voodoo Science, Park
    (2000) puts it simply: we have been bamboozled. The woozle itself arises from a pattern of
    events and circumstances, not from any single factor. According to Gelles (1980), one of the
    most important factors was frequently citing one or two studies in different publications,
    even when those particular studies were methodologically flawed and drew questionable
    conclusions. But, as will be described shortly, other researchers have described many paths
    through which the data from one or two studies can be manipulated, misperceived, and
    mishandled in ways that create woozles.
    A Famous Divorce Woozle
    In the social sciences one of the most well-known examples of a woozle arose from a single
    study by the sociologist, Lenore Weitzman (1985), which was widely disseminated in her
    bestselling book, The Divorce Revolution: The Unexpected Social and Economic
    Consequences for Women and America.
    The woozle was this: Weitzman claimed that most American women suffer a 73% decline in
    their standard of living after a divorce, while their ex-husbands enjoy a 42% increase. The
    ground was fertile for the growth of this particular woozle because Weitzman’s book was
    published at a time when the public was worried about the rising divorce rates and about the
    possible negative impact that the new and controversial “no-fault” divorce laws might have
    on women.
    Although her conclusion was based solely on her one study with 228 people from Los
    Angeles, and although her data were at odds with the prevailing social science research and
    government statistics, her message “went viral” as we would say today. In part the study was
    widely accepted because it confirmed what many people already believed: Most men exploit
    their ex-wives financially. For more than a decade, the study maintained its popularity and its
    influence.
    The book was reviewed in at least 22 social science journals, 12 law reviews, and 10 national
    magazines—and was cited over 25 times in national magazines, in at least 24 legal cases in
    state appellate and supreme courts and once in the U.S. Supreme Court. It received the
    American Sociological Association’s 1986 Book Award. From 1986 to 1993, the book was
    cited in 348 social science articles and in more than 250 law review articles as evidence that
    divorce laws needed to be changed because they discriminated against women financially
    (Sommers, 1994).
    Some scholars immediately began to question Weitzman’s (1985) conclusions and asked her
    to provide the raw data for replication. It took more than a decade for Weitzman to admit
    that her conclusions had been wrong. Researchers who tried to reconstruct her database
    found that women’s standards of living declined by 27% (not 73%) and that men’s increased
    by 10% (not 42%; Peterson, 1996a; Peterson, 1996b; Weitzman, 1996). Keep in mind that
    the defining hallmark of a woozle is not whether the authors of the original study made
    errors in analyzing their data or whether they misrepresented their data.
    The Weitzman study exemplifies a woozle because seriously flawed data from a single study
    became an international sensation and had a widespread impact on divorce laws and on
    public opinion. It is the process by which Weitzman’s limited and flawed data became so
    widely accepted and so influential that characterizes this study as one of the more infamous
    examples of a woozle in social science. Keeping the Weitzman (1985) woozle in mind, the
    question is whether something similar might be occurring in regard to the current debates
    over custody law reform and parenting plans for children under the age of 5 years. Among
    others, Johnston (2007) noted that family law is an area in which woozling might be
    especially likely to occur:
    “Distortions and misuses of social science data in family law matters derive partly from the
    political nature of the issues and from gender wars” (p. 16).
    How Are Woozles Born and Raised?
    Before illustrating how one particular woozle has arisen in regard to child custody, we have
    to be able to recognize the manyways in which woozling occurs. How do studies become
    part of a woozle ? How do data from a study get misrepresented into something that barely
    resembles the researchers’ original findings? As we will see, no one person or no one event
    can be held accountable for creating or for promoting a woozle.
    The process involves a constellation of factors, interacting with one another in ways that
    often are unpredictable and unforeseen. As described below, academicians have expanded
    on Gelles (1980) original ideas about how woozles are created – describing numerous ways
    in which data become distorted into woozles. Many are beyond the control of the authors of
    the original study, whereas others clearly involve their intentional or unintentional
    participation.
    Evidence by Citation
    According to Gelles’s (1980) original description of woozles, they often began when one or
    two studies are frequently and repeatedly cited in articles or at conferences and seminars –
    while the bulk of the research is ignored or rarely mentioned. The more frequently the study
    is cited, the more credible it becomes. In other words, people naively assume that just
    because a particular study is being widely cited and discussed, its conclusions are valid and
    reliable. Gelles (1980) referred to this process as “evidence by citation.”
    Misrepresenting Other Researchers’ Data
    Woozles also are linked to professionals’ discussing and writing about a study’s findings
    without ever having read it—or having read only a synopsis or an abstract. In this way, the
    data become misrepresented and studies are cited in support of positions that are directly
    opposite to their conclusions. The sociologist Johnston (2007) called these
    misrepresentations “scholarly rumors” – erroneous beliefs that arise when scholars
    misquote data from a study and then quote one another without checking back to the
    original source. For instance, the authors of the original study may have stated clearly that
    their data were not statistically significant and that their conclusions were speculative and
    inconclusive. But the data can end up being reported as unequivocal and significant. Another
    way of misrepresenting data is to report several studies as having reached similar
    conclusions, when in fact they did not. This buttresses the findings of a single study by
    creating the impression that there is an emerging consensus, a pattern, or a trend, when
    there is not.
    Cherry Picking
    Moreover, writers or speakers may choose to report only a few studies or only some of the
    findings from a particular study – a bias referred to as “cherry picking” (Johnston, 2007). For
    example, articles claiming to be reviews of the literature may report very little of the existing
    data, choosing only to report those studies that support one view. Johnston (2007) also
    noted another version of cherry picking: Researchers’ acknowledging the limitations of their
    own study when writing in professional journals or speaking to other researchers— but not
    when discussing their study with the media or with audiences where other researchers are
    not there to challenge them. In other words, the researchers take a far more moderate
    position in the presence of scholars who are knowledgeable on the topic than they do with
    audiences who are more naïve and more easily misled. By cherry picking, only those studies
    or only those interpretations of ambiguous and contradictory data that support the woozle
    are presented.
    The White Hat Bias
    Another version of cherry picking is the “white hat bias” – a phrase coined by public health
    researchers Cope and Allison (Cope & Allison, 2010) to describe the bias in reporting the data
    on soft drinks and obesity. As with the ‘good guys’ wearing the white hats in the cowboy
    movies, well-intentioned authors can be biased in reporting the research because they are
    trying to achieve a “righteous end.” For example, in their meta-analysis, Cope and Allison
    found that the impact of consuming sugared drinks was far smaller than reported in the
    academic literature on obesity. Attributing this error to the white hat bias, Cope and Allison
    urged authors to be more humble and urged journals to set higher standards when reporting
    the research literature.
    Confirmation Bias
    A woozle also is more likely to arise and to spread when it confirms beliefs that people
    already hold—an effect known as “confirmation bias” (Chabris & Simons, 2010). We are
    overly critical and dismissive of data that contradict our existing beliefs and are too willing to
    accept data that confirm them. Confirmation bias might be especially relevant in research
    related to child custody because most people have strong beliefs and feelings related to
    gender roles and parenting. For example, people may believe that females have a maternal
    instinct that makes them better suited than males to raising children. This belief, in turn,
    would tend to make them more receptive to believing research studies – or woozles – that
    confirm that particular belief.
    Researchers’ Contributions to Woozling Their Data
    Researchers themselves can also inadvertently— or in some cases intentionally— contribute
    to the woozling of their data. For example, when presenting their findings, researchers might
    not report the data that contradicted their hypothesis. Or the researchers might exaggerate
    the significance of their data, present their findings in ways that are misleading, put
    disproportionate emphasis on some of their findings while ignoring others, or make policy
    recommendations that overreach their findings.
    They might also frame their research questions and interpret their data to support the
    desired conclusions or from only one theoretical position. Researchers also might create
    their own measures, use unorthodox or invalid procedures, or rely on abridged versions of
    standardized scales that have no established validity or reliability. As a consequence, data
    that are uninterpretable or ambiguous are presented as if they are valid and reliable.
    Researchers also might minimize or ignore, rather than acknowledge and address, the
    ambiguous or contradictory data in their study.
    Or they might generalize their findings to populations that have little in common with the
    sample in their study. Finally, researchers might mislead people to believe that their study
    was based on large numbers of people when in fact it was not. For example, they can inflate
    the numbers by alluding to the total number of people in the large database from which
    their data were taken, diverting attention away from the fact that the actual samples in their
    study were much smaller.
    Researchers may also contribute to the woozling of their own data by trying to silence or to
    demean their critics. One technique is to claim they are being unfairly “picked on” by their
    peers, or that other scholars are “conspiring” against them or trying to “polarize” people
    (Park, 2000).
    Another technique is to try to discredit critics by attacking their character—for example,
    accusing them of being fathers’ rights activists or feminists (Johnston, 2007). Another way to
    discredit critics is to present oneself as the “protector” of or the “spokesperson” for the
    disadvantaged, the victimized, or the weak. For example, in regard to custody issues, some
    researchers might claim that they are the ones who are speaking on behalf of the children
    and who are putting the children’s needs first—implying that those researchers who do not
    share their point of view are not equally concerned about the children and are foolishly
    putting the parents’ needs and wishes ahead of the children’s needs.
    The sociologist, Joel Best, wrote extensively about the misuse of statistics and research that
    is part of the woozling process (Best, 2001, 2008, 2013). In regard to researchers’ biases, he
    explained (Best, 2001) that we mistakenly assume that research data:
    ” . . . simply exist, like rocks, completely independent of people; and that people gather
    statistics much as rock collectors pick up stones. This is wrong. All statistics are created
    through people’s actions: people have to decide what to count and how to count it. People
    have to do the counting and have to interpret the results and to decide what the numbers
    mean.” (p. 27)
    Given this, Best (2001) suggested we ask ourselves: Is the author of this study someone who
    has any stake in its outcomes? In a similar vein, researchers may inadvertently contribute to
    the woozling of their data through the media. For example, Park (2000) noted that physicists
    sometimes contributed to distortions of their data by presenting their findings directly to the
    media and by using dramatic anecdotal stories to make their data more memorable.
    It is ironic that academicians may inadvertently contribute to other researchers’ woozles by
    not being engaged enough with the media. Lilienfeld (2012) who wrote extensively on how
    flawed social science data contributed to popular myths, pointed out that most university
    researchers were reluctant to devote their time to disseminating data through the media,
    even though doing so would help to combat the most flawed data that tended to attract the
    most media attention.
    Not only do most universities not reward this type of public service, disseminating research
    through the media is often frowned on by academicians. The unfortunate result is that the
    media end up having to rely on data from people who are not full-time university
    researchers which, in turn, can contribute to woozles (Lilienfeld, 2012).
    Compelling Stories, Confidence, and Credentials
    Both Best (2001) and Kahneman (2011), also remind us that how the data were presented
    can contribute to distortions. For example, when a study is presented along with compelling,
    personal stories, we are more likely to remember it, to repeat it, and to believe it.
    These anecdotal stories or dramatic case studies are often exaggerated and generally not
    representative of the problem at hand (Best, 2001). Having aroused people’s emotions,
    these techniques increase the odds that the data will be more widely disseminated and, in
    too many cases, more widely woozled (Kahneman, 2011). For example, an actress who
    repeatedly claimed on TV that her child had become autistic after being vaccinated was cited
    by some parents as the reason for not vaccinating their children, despite the fact that the
    scientific literature shows no connection between autism and vaccinations (Chabris &
    Simons, 2010).
    The use of dramatic anecdotes or emotionally laden personal stories is a well-known
    technique in the art of persuasion, often used by politicians, for example. Unfortunately
    anecdotal stories and case studies can contribute to people’s misunderstandings of the data,
    especially if the data are related to controversial issues such as child custody. Case studies
    and anecdotes also make it easier for advocacy groups to misuse data to suit their own
    agenda.
    Given this, researchers can reduce the odds of their data being woozled by presenting case
    studies or anecdotes only within the context of empirical data—and by steering clear of
    dramatic stories that might convey an unbalanced view of unresolved issues or ambiguous
    data.
    Cognitive psychologists also have demonstrated that we are more likely to believe data
    presented by a confident or a well-known person (Chabris & Simons, 2010). For example, we
    find data more credible coming from people with important sounding titles or prestigious
    institutional affiliations: “Dr. X, executive director of the prestigious Y institute and one of
    the world’s experts on . . . explains that . . .” (Johnston, 2007, p. 18).
    Simple Explanations and Neuroscience
    We are also more likely to accept data that offer relatively simple explanations for
    complicated questions (Kahneman, 2011).
    Our preference for simplicity may be one of the reasons why it has become increasingly
    popular to cite neuroscience data to buttress the findings of social science studies (Lilienfeld,
    2012).
    As Lilienfeld (2012) explained, neuroscience data are appealing because they tend to be
    relatively simple and because we assume they are more objective and more “scientific” than
    social science data. For example, experimental subjects were more likely to accept the
    findings of a social science study as true when the words “brain scans indicate” or other
    phrases from neuroscience were included in the description.
    Even when the interpretations of the data were illogical, and even though there was no
    demonstrated link between the behavior under study and the neurological data, the subjects
    were more willing to accept the results of the social science study (Weisberg, 2008). This
    finding alerts us to the possibility that when social scientists invoke neuroscience data in
    support of their study’s findings, their data might garner more credibility than it actually
    warrants—and might consequently be more easily incorporated into an existing woozle.
    Policy Recommendations and Organizations’ Guidelines
    The final hallmark of a woozle is that the limited, ambiguous, or flawed data that underlie it
    become the basis for public policies and guidelines for professional organizations. In a recent
    American Psychologist article (Rosik, Jones, & Byrd, 2012), the authors pointed out that
    accuracy and precision in reporting data are of utmost importance when those findings are
    being used to write guidelines or to make pronouncements that affect practitioners.
    No organization’s guidelines or policy recommendations should be based on only a few
    studies or issued in the absence of conclusive evidence. When policies or guidelines are
    based on only a few studies, we have reason to suspect that woozling may be at work.
    Characteristics of Woozles
    To summarize, many scholars in the social sciences and in other disciplines have written
    extensively about the ways in which data can become distorted into woozles. Among the
    most common processes that have been discussed by these scholars are the following:
    ▪ In articles and in seminars a few studies are cherry-picked to support one position.
    ▪ Two or three studies are repeatedly cited and discussed as “the research” on a topic.
    ▪ Reviews of the research, especially those making policy recommendations, are based
    primarily on the same few studies, ignoring the bulk of the research.
    ▪ The data are often presented in dramatic ways with anecdotal stories, case studies,
    or emotionally laden pictures and graphics.
    ▪ The significance of the findings are overstated while the limitations are understated.
    ▪ Data from small or non-representative samples are generalized to the general
    population.
    ▪ Only one theoretical perspective is used to frame the question and interpret the
    data.
    ▪ The data are based on measures with no established reliability or validity.
    ▪ Media reports, synopses, abstracts, or press releases overstate or misrepresent the
    actual data in the study.
    ▪ Data that are not statistically significant or that are contradictory and ambiguous are
    reported as important.
    ▪ The authors promote their own study as a basis for a particular position without
    putting their data in the context of the larger body of evidence.
    ▪ Studies are presented together as if they reached the same conclusion, when in fact
    they did not.
    Woozles Versus Data: How to Convince Winnie There Is No Woozle
    In regard to the current debates over custody law reform and parenting plans, the woozle
    we are going to examine is this: Infants and children 4 years and younger who spend
    overnight time in their fathers’ care are more irritable, more severely distressed and
    insecure in their relationships with their mothers, more poorly behaved with their peers,
    more stressed and thus more likely to wheeze, more easily distracted (less persistent), and
    more likely to have trouble regulating their emotions. In short, overnighting has a
    deleterious impact on infants and other children under the age of 4 years.
    This particular woozle attracts the attention of the public, policymakers, and family court
    professionals for at least two reasons.
    First it relates to two controversial custody questions: How much time should infants and
    preschoolers spend with each parent after their separation? Do children this young need to
    sleep in the same home every night and, if not, how many nights should they spend in each
    parent’s care? More specific, at what age are shared parenting plans (35%–50% time with
    each parent) appropriate for children?
    Second, this woozle arouses many people’s emotions because it affirms a belief that some
    people hold dear, but that others find insulting, outdated, and irrational: the belief that
    women are better suited than men to raise children— especially infants and toddlers. While
    the woozle appeals to people who believe females have a maternal instinct or neurological
    structures in their brains that better equip them to bond and communicate with infants, it
    offends people who believe that men and women are equally capable of parenting their
    children, including their infants.
    The woozle is related to two separate custody issues: overnighting and shared parenting. The
    term overnighting is typically used only when referring to infants and to preschool children.
    Overnighting literally means how many nights these very young children spend away from
    their mother in their father’s care.
    When the number of nights that children (of any age) spend with their fathers exceeds 30%,
    the parenting plan is generally referred to as “shared parenting,” “shared care,” or “shared
    residence.” In older studies shared parenting plans were referred to as joint or shared
    physical custody. In contrast, the terms primary care or sole custody are used
    interchangeably when children live primarily – or exclusively – with their mother and spend
    less than 30% of the time with their father.
    Typically primary care/sole custody parenting plans allot only two weekends a month (4–6
    overnights) and one midweek visit of a few hours to the father, for a total of roughly 20% of
    the parenting time. Unlike these social science definitions, the legal definitions of shared
    care/shared parenting/shared physical custody that are used to determine child support
    payment vary from country to country and from state to state.
    To determine whether this statement is a woozle and, if it is, to challenge it, we have to
    know the results of the other studies that have gathered data about children under the age
    of 5 years whose parenting plans involved overnighting. We also need a description of the
    samples to know which findings are applicable to the general population of divorced parents
    and which are not.
    There are currently 31 studies that have compared the ‘outcomes’ of children who live in
    shared parenting families (30% – 50% of the time) to children who live with their mother and
    spend varying amounts of overnight time with their father. Only eight of these 31 studies
    have included infants and children under the age of 6 years.
    Of those nine, only four focused exclusively on infants and children under the age of 5 years.
    More detailed discussions of most of these studies are available elsewhere (Lamb, 2012a;
    Pruett, Cowan, Cowan, & Diamond, 2012). But a brief summary shows how little support
    these nine studies lend to the woozle that spending overnight time in their fathers’ care has
    a deleterious impact on infants and toddlers.
    The studies are presented in three distinct groups: all formerly married parents, largely
    formerly married parents, and rarely formerly married. There are likely to be significant
    differences between these three groups in terms of socioeconomic variables, age, ethnicity,
    longevity of their relationship, and factors such as incarceration, poverty, and parenting skills
    that are generally associated with poor outcomes for children.
    For these reasons, overlooking or minimizing these differences in the research studies can
    contribute to inappropriate custody decisions and to misguided recommendations regarding
    custody law reform. Data from studies with high numbers of never married parents,
    especially when many of them were not even living together when their child was born,
    should not be applied to formerly married parents who were raising their child together
    before their separation.
    The Formerly Married
    Three of the eight studies only included parents who had formerly been married, meaning
    these data are the most applicable to divorced parents. The first study compared 58 children
    who lived with their mother and 35 who lived at least 35% time with their father, with half of
    them being 4-years-old or younger (Kline, Tschann, Johnston, & Wallerstein, 1989).
    One to 2 years after their parents’ separation, there were no differences in social or behavior
    adjustment between the two groups. The frequent overnighters, however, had better
    relationships with their fathers and were better adjusted emotionally.
    The second study, the Stanford Custody Project, followed children from 1,100 divorced
    families in California over a period of 4 years (Maccoby & Mnookin, 1992). What made this
    study so unique for its time was that the children in 150 of these families were overnighting
    30% to 50% time with their fathers. In these families, 125 of the children were infants or
    preschoolers younger than 5 years.
    At the end of 4 years, the frequently overnighting children were better off than the others on
    all of the standardized measures of their academic, emotional, physical, and behavioral well-
    being. Three years after the parents’ divorce, only 1.6% of the frequent overnighters’ fathers
    were seeing less of their children compared to 56% of the other fathers.
    The third study assessed children from nearly 600 shared parenting and 600 primary care
    families in Wisconsin (Melli & Brown, 2008). Roughly 40% of the children were under the age
    of 5 years. Three years after their parents’ divorce, the frequently overnighting (35% – 50%
    time) children had better relationships with their fathers, were happier and less depressed,
    and had fewer health problems than the less frequently overnighting children.
    There were no differences on measures of emotional health. Moreover, 82% of the frequent
    overnighters’ fathers were spending just as much time with their children as they had 3 years
    earlier, in contrast to only 55% of the other fathers.
    Majority Formerly Married
    In three other studies the majority, but not all, of the parents had been married before
    separating. A sizable minority had separated before the child was born; and others had never
    lived together at all.
    The first study merits careful attention because it is so frequently and mistakenly cited as
    evidence that overnighting interferes with infants’ attachments to their mothers. The
    limitations of this study have been pointed out by a number of scholars (Cashmore &
    Parkinson, 2011; Lamb & Kelly, 2001; Pruett et al., 2012; Warshak, 2002). Because a sizable
    minority of the parents had never been married or lived together, many of the infants had
    no relationship with their fathers before the overnighting began.
    Moreover, all of the infants, even in the married families, had exceptionally high levels of
    disorganized attachments. Then too, the overnighting infants’ parents were far more
    combative, less communicative, more violent, more likely never to have lived together, and
    more likely to have children from several different relationships than the parents of infants
    who did not overnight.
    For many reasons then, caution should be exercised before generalizing these data to
    separated parents in the general population.
    The researchers compared infants 12- to 20-months-old in three types of families: 52 in
    intact families, 49 who never overnighted, and 44 who occasionally overnighted (Solomon &
    George, 1999).
    Only nine of the 44 overnighters spent more than four nights a month with their father. The
    only factors assessed were how securely the infants were attached to each parent and, 1
    year later, how well the toddlers performed on a challenging task with their mother in a
    laboratory playroom. Compared to non-overnighters, the overnighters were no less securely
    attached to their mothers:
    “Neither the particular pattern of overnight visits nor the total amount of time away from
    the mother predicted disorganized attachment. Insecure attachment in the overnight group
    was associated with high parent conflict and low parent communication” (Solomon, 2013, p.
    269).
    The fact that the overnighters had more disorganized attachments than infants in married
    families was attributed to the fact that their parents had so much more verbal and physical
    conflict and much worse communication than the non-overnighters’ parents.
    In the second part of the study 1 year later, the overnighters did as well as the non-
    overnighters on the challenging task with their mothers. There was no way to determine
    whether the overnighting toddlers were more distressed than the non-overnighters when
    separated from their mothers because these two groups were never compared. The non-
    overnighting and the intact family toddlers were combined into one group. Almost one third
    (27%) of this “combined” group were upset after a second brief separation from their
    mothers in the laboratory, compared to 50% of the overnighting infants.
    This finding was difficult to interpret because the overnighters and nonovernighters were
    not directly compared and because some overnighters had only recently begun overnighting,
    while others had been overnighting for a year. Solomon and George (1999) also cautioned
    that the infants’ behavior in a contrived situation in a laboratory playroom should not be
    generalized to shared parenting families:
    We tentatively conclude that overnight visitation schedules can disorganize the child’s
    attachment strategies, but that such disorganization does not necessarily pervade or reflect
    the overall quality of the mother-child relationship . . . Whereas there is now considerable
    research demonstrating poor developmental outcomes for children in normative and other
    kinds of high risk samples who are classified as insecure-disorganized, based on our finding,
    the same prognosis should not necessarily apply to disorganized and unclassifiable children
    who are participating in overnight visitation schedules. (Solomon & George, 1999, p. 258)
    Even more recently Solomon (2013) reiterated the study’s original conclusions: “When
    parents have open lines of communication about their infant, there is little or no reason to
    be concerned about the long term developmental outcome for such children” (p. 276). In
    sum, this study did not find a significant link between overnighting and difficulties in
    emotional regulation or insecure attachments.
    In the second study (Pruett, Ebling, & Insabella, 2004) the researchers assessed 132 children
    between the ages of 2 and 6 years on several standardized measures of well-being. Most
    (75%) overnighted at least once weekly, even though 25% of their parents had not been
    married before separating (Pruett, Ebling, & Insabella, 2004).
    For the 2- and 3-year-olds, the overnighters were no different from non-overnighters in
    regard to: sleep problems, depression, anxiety, aggression, or social withdrawal. Their
    fathers, but not their mothers, said the overnighting toddlers were somewhat more irritable.
    For the 4- to 6-year-olds, especially for the girls, the overnighters were better off in regard to
    attention problems and social withdrawal and were no different from the non-overnighters
    on the other measures. It is also worth noting that having several different adults taking care
    of them throughout the day had no negative impact on the infants or toddlers, but having an
    inconsistent, erratic parenting schedule did—especially for the boys.
    In the third study involving 7,118 separated Australian parents, only 50% had formerly been
    married and 12% never lived together – meaning more caution should be exercised before
    applying these findings to divorced parents (Kaspiew et al., 2009).
    For children under age 4 years, there were 3,513 children overnighting less than 35% time
    and 480 overnighting 35% to 50% time, 201 under the age of years, and 266 ages 3 to 4
    years. The mothers reported no differences between the two groups of children on
    measures of physical health or socio-emotional well-being. In contrast, the fathers of the
    frequent overnighters rated their children higher on health, learning skills, and overall
    progress than the other fathers. Overall the frequent overnighters had marginally better
    outcomes, even after accounting for parents’ levels of violence, conflict, and education.
    More important, violence between the parents had no worse impact on the frequent
    overnighters than on the other children.
    Never Married Parents
    The last two studies are distinct because so few of the parents had been married and so
    many had never lived together. The one study (McIntosh, Smyth, Kelaher, & Wells, 2010) will
    be discussed in the next section because it forms the basis of the woozle.
    The final study (Tornello et al., 2013) should not be generalized to divorced parents or to the
    vast majority of never married parents because the data were from the Fragile Families and
    Child Wellbeing database (McClanahan, 2011). All of these parents lived in the inner cities of
    America’s 20 largest cities; 65% had no high school degree; 85% were African or Hispanic
    American; and 60% were below the poverty level.
    Slightly more than 85% were not married when their children were born. Of these, 30% were
    not living together and 20% no longer had a relationship with each other when their child
    was born. Before their children’s fifth birthday, 50% of these fathers and 10% of these
    mothers have served time in jail (Center for Research on Child Wellbeing, 2013).
    For these reasons, any study using this database should take care not to generalize the
    findings to families who do not fit this unique profile—not even to other never married or
    impoverished Americans.
    Using the Fragile Families database, the study (Tornello et al., 2013) examined two samples
    taken 2 years apart: 384 one-year olds and 608 three-year-olds who overnighted were
    compared to 1,062 who did not overnight and had infrequent daytime contact with their
    fathers. For the infants, occasional overnights meant anywhere from one to 51 nights a year;
    and frequent meant anywhere from 51 to 256 nights. The toddlers were categorized
    differently: rare overnights meant one to 12 nights a year, occasional meant 12 to 127
    nights, and frequent meant 128 to 256 nights.
    Consistent with the seven studies already described, there were virtually no differences
    between the overnighters and non-overnighters.
    On 14 regression analyses for the seven measures of well-being, only one statistically
    significant difference emerged:
    The children who frequently overnighted at age 3 years displayed more
    positive behavior at age 5 years than the rare or no overnights groups. In
    regard to children’s attachments to their mothers, based on reports from only
    60% of the mothers, the 51 frequently overnighting infants had more insecure
    attachments (43%) than the 219 occasional overnighters (16%) and the 364
    non-overnighters (25%).
    However, in contrast to the hypothesis that overnighting would be linked to insecure
    attachments, the infants who never overnighted were more insecure than infants who
    occasionally overnighted. The data also failed to support the attachment hypothesis for the
    3-year-olds. The 60 frequent and 171 rare overnighters had virtually the same ratings (37%,
    33% insecure, respectively), as did the 171 occasional and 320 non-overnighters (22%, 18%
    insecure, respectively).
    Even if there had been a clear pattern between overnighting and the attachment ratings,
    interpreting the data would have been problematic for several reasons, some of which have
    been noted in a recent critique of the study (Milar & Kruk, in press).
    First and foremost, regardless of how frequently they overnighted, these infants and
    toddlers did not have alarmingly high rates of insecurity compared to children from similar
    backgrounds in the general population.
    On the Toddler Attachment Q Sort (TAQ), which was an abbreviated version of the
    standardized Attachment Q Sort (AQS; Waters & Deane, 1985), in the general population,
    49% of infants and toddlers who were living in poverty, or who were African American, or
    who had mothers without high school degrees were rated as insecurely attached—a number
    that increased dramatically to 61% insecure attachments for children younger than 21-
    months-old (Andreassen & Fletcher, 2007).
    Second, 26 of the 51 infants and 45 of the 60 toddlers in the frequent overnights group were
    actually living with their father 55% to 70% of the time. These children should not have been
    included in an analysis of attachment because their mothers were not providing most of
    their care. In that vein, many of these infants and toddlers may have been living mainly with
    their fathers because their mothers had psychological, behavioral, or substance abuse
    problems—the types of problems that would undermine secure attachments independent of
    overnighting.
    But the greater problem is that the attachment data came from the mothers’ ratings on the
    TAQ. Unfortunately, in a meta-analysis of 139 studies with 13,835 children, the AQS was only
    found to be valid when trained observers did the rating after observing the mother and child
    interact for several hours:
    “It is concluded that the observer AQS, but not the self-reportedAQS, is a valid
    measure of attachment” (van IJzendoorn, Vereijken, Kranenburg, & Walraven,
    2004, p. 1188).
    “The convergent and discriminant validity of the self-reported AQS does not
    yet warrant its use as a measure of attachment security” (van IJzendoorn et
    al., 2004, p. 1206).
    Waters (2013) who developed the AQS also expressed his concern over mothers’ ratings:
    “I am embarrassed to say that I was surprised when most of the people who
    contacted me wanted to have mothers do the sorting.”
    “If you are interested in correlations, I would avoid mothers” (Waters, 2013, p.
    1).
    Unfortunately, because observer ratings would have been too expensive, the TAQ ratings in
    the Fragile Families study had to be done by the mothers. As a result, it was not clear what
    was being measured by the TAQ scores in this study.
    This problem has been acknowledged by other researchers who have used the TAQ data
    from the Fragile Family database (Pudasainee-Kapri & Razza, 2013). Overall then,
    overnighting had one positive impact and no negative impact on the well-being of these
    infants and toddlers.
    In sum, the woozle finds little, if any, support in seven of these eight studies. It is also
    important to note that three of the eight studies (McIntosh et al., 2010; Solomon & George,
    1999; Tornello et al., 2013) were predicated on assumptions about mother–infant
    attachment that many contemporary attachment researchers and recent empirical studies
    do not support.
    First, these three studies assume that infants form one “primary” attachment to only one of
    their parents; second, that the quality (security) of this one relationship largely determines
    infants’ abilities to regulate their emotions; third, that this attachment takes precedent over
    the father – infant bond especially in the first year of the infant’s life; and fourth, that
    overnight time away from the mother, unlike daytime separation, is particularly stressful and
    undermines the security of their attachment. For these reasons, these three studies assumed
    that infant–mother attachment should be a primary measure of infants’ well-being and the
    central focus of parenting plans.
    In fact, however, many researchers do not agree with these assumptions about attachment
    largely because they are not consistent with recent empirical data (Cashmore & Parkinson,
    2011; Garber, 2012; Hynan, 2012; Lamb, 2012a; Ludolph, 2012; Ludolph & Dale, 2012;
    Warshak, 2012).
    The woozle is further undermined by the consensus of a large group of social scientists:
    “No sufficient evidence exists to support postponing the introduction of regular and frequent
    involvement, including overnights, of both parents with their babies and toddlers. The
    theoretical and practical considerations favoring overnights for most young children are
    more compelling than concerns that overnights might jeopardize children’s development”
    (Warshak, in press).
    Goals and Outcomes of the Woozled Study
    The woozle that overnighting causes a host of problems for infants and toddlers—notably,
    undermining their secure attachments to their mothers—is largely based on one study that
    has captured more of the public’s attention and exerted more influence than the seven
    studies previously discussed.
    The study, which will be referred to as the “preschooler study” to distinguish it from a
    second study that was published in the same document, was part of a report commissioned
    by the Australian Attorney General’s office (McIntosh et al., 2010).
    The central question of the preschooler study was: What impact does spending overnight
    time in their father’s care have on infants and other children 5-years-old and under?
    (Because 95% of the non-residential parents were fathers, they will be referred as such). The
    underlying policy question was: ‘Should custody laws allow or should parenting plans include
    overnights for children this young?’
    Again, it is important to keep in mind that these researchers approached the question of
    overnighting from only one perspective: that infants form a “primary” attachment to only
    one parent and later form a “secondary” attachment to their other parent.
    The study was also based on the hypothesis that being separated from their mothers
    overnight was especially stressful for infants, making it more difficult for them to be securely
    attached to her and to regulate their emotions. “In this light our core question was whether
    the frequency of overnight care was linked to emotional regulation and stress in infants and
    young children” (McIntosh et al., 2010, p. 143). Although the researchers clearly stated that
    they only framed their questions and only interpreted their data through this particular
    “attachment lens,” the woozle fails to acknowledge this important limitation.
    The data for the pre-schooler study were taken from the Longitudinal Study of Australian
    Children (LSAC) database, an on-going national survey that, at that time, had collected data
    on almost 10,000 children (AIFS, 2012). The pre-schooler study, however, only included the
    2,052 children under the age of 5 years whose parents were separated. But because half of
    these children never spent any overnight time in their father’s care and because many
    overnighting children were not assessed on all of the measures, the sample sizes were often
    quite small.
    For example, there were as few as 14 and never more than 20 infants in the occasional
    overnights group on all six measures. These small sample sizes are important because, as we
    will see, the study is sometimes presented in ways that imply the data came from thousands
    of overnighting children. In fact, the negative data on which the woozle is based came from
    some of the smallest samples in the study.
    The pre-schooler study compared three age groups: infants under 2 years, 2- to 3-year-olds,
    and 4- to 5-year-olds. The three family types were:
    No overnights,
    Occasional overnights (1–3 nights monthly for infants and 1–9 nights for the 2- to 5-year-
    olds) and shared care (4–15 nights monthly for infants and 10–15 nights for 2- to 5-year-
    olds).
    It is especially important to note that “shared care” (the terminology used for “shared
    parenting” in this study) for the infants was not defined as 30% to 50% time, as is defined in
    the literature. Because there were only 11 infants who were in their fathers’ care 35% to
    50% of the time, these researchers decided to define shared care as spending as few as four
    nights a month with their father.
    As with the Tornello et al. (2013) study, the study focused primarily on parents who had not
    been married or living together before separating – 30% of whom had never lived together.
    The small number of married parents is important because, as we will see, the study is often
    cited as evidence against overnighting and shared parenting for all parents – not just for
    parents who have never been married or never lived together.
    According to the 15 page synopsis of the 169 page report, the overall impact of overnighting
    for children ages zero to 4 years – even overnighting as little as once a week—was largely
    negative (McIntosh et al., 2010):
    Young infants under two years of age living with a nonresident parent for only one or more
    nights a week were more irritable, and were more watchful and wary of separation from
    their primary caregiver than those primarily in the care of one parent. Children aged 2–3
    years in shared care . . . showed significantly lower levels of persistence with routine tasks,
    learning and play than children in the other two groups. Of concern, but as predicted by
    attachment theory, they also showed severely distressed behaviors in their relationship with
    the primary parent (often very upset, crying or hanging onto the parent and hitting, biting or
    kicking) feeding related problems (gagging on food or refusing to eat) and not reacting when
    hurt. Such behaviors are consistent with high levels of attachment distress . . .. Thus,
    regardless of socioeconomic background, parenting or inter parental cooperation, shared
    overnight care of children under four years of age had an independent and deleterious
    impact on several emotional and behavioral regulation outcomes. (p. 9)
    It is also important to know that there were no significant differences between the 4- and 5-
    year-olds in the different overnighting groups, which is why the woozle restricts itself to
    children 4 years and younger.
    The preschooler study’s researchers (McIntosh et al., 2010) reinforced the study’s
    conclusions with the views of the neuroscientist, Allan Schore (Schore & McIntosh, 2011),
    who believes that female brains are neurologically equipped for communicating with and
    forming attachments to infants. Schore further states that:
    “Science suggests that one primary caregiver needs to be the constant source
    of bedtime routines” (Schore & McIntosh, 2011, p. 508).
    Contributions of the Study
    As already noted, a study is not without merit merely because its data have become part of a
    woozle. The preschooler study has made several contributions to the field. It has revived
    interest in looking more carefully at how parenting plans affect children at very young ages.
    It also proposed factors related to children’s well-being and parenting plans that need to be
    examined in future research. Given its focus on attachment theory, it is of particular interest
    to researchers in that field.
    Because the frequency of overnighting was considered, more detailed information could be
    gathered about the linear effects of overnighting. Moreover, the researchers focused mainly
    on children whose parents had never been married to one another – and in many cases had
    never lived together.
    This underscores the importance of gathering data about an increasingly large group of
    children who are born out of wedlock and whose parents live together only briefly, if at all.
    By raising many unresolved and controversial issues, the study serves as a reminder that
    more research is needed on overnight parenting plans for the youngest children.
    Limitations of the Study: What the Woozle Ignores
    One of the fundamental features of a woozle is that it ignores or minimizes the limitations of
    those studies on which it is founded.
    Even when the researchers themselves have pointed out the limitations of their study, the
    woozle ignores them. Since the publication of the report in which the preschooler study first
    appeared (McIntosh et al., 2010), a number of social scientists have noted the study’s
    shortcomings and have cautioned against generalizing its results or using its data as the basis
    for parenting plan recommendations (Cashmore & Parkinson, 2011; Lamb, 2012b; Ludolph &
    Dale, 2012; Nielsen, 2013a; Nielsen, 2013b; Parkinson & Cashmore, 2011; Pruett et al., 2012;
    Warshak, 2012).
    Understanding these limitations helps us appreciate how large the gap is between the
    woozle and the actual data in the study. To begin, the preschooler study was limited by the
    sample, the sample sizes, and its atypical definition of shared care for infants.
    Most of these parents had never been married to one another (90% for infants and 60% for
    toddlers) and 30% of the infants’ parents had never even lived together. This means the
    findings should not be generalized to the general population of divorced parents. And in
    contrast to all other studies where shared care/shared parenting is defined as 35% to 50%
    time sharing, this study categorized the 48 infants who spent as few as four nights a month
    in their father’s care as being in shared care.
    The researchers did this because there were only 11 infants who actually were spending 35%
    or more time overnighting. As a result of this unusual way of categorizing the infants, there
    was no way to assess the impact of overnighting only once a week versus overnighting more
    frequently – and no way to assess the impact of shared care as it is always defined in the
    literature. Another limitation was that the number of overnighting infants was very small on
    many measures.
    For example, in the occasional overnight group, there were as few as 14 and no more than
    20 infants measured on any of the six outcomes. Of greater concern and in contrast to the
    woozle’s claim that overnighting as little as once a week had a negative impact, this study
    never compared the children who never overnighted to the children who only occasionally
    overnighted. That is, the study never addressed the question: ‘Is occasional overnighting
    better or worse than never overnighting?’
    Further limiting the study, there was no established validity or reliability for four of the six
    measures: irritability, persistence, wheezing, and wariness/watchfulness about the mother’s
    whereabouts (AIFS, 2012). Unfortunately without reported validity and reliability on these
    adapted versions of standardized measures, the data cannot be interpreted with any
    confidence.
    This problem is especially noteworthy because these are the four measures that form the
    basis of the woozle’s claim that overnighting creates physical stress, emotional regulation
    difficulties, lack of persistence, and wariness/watchfulness in regard to he mother’s
    presence.
    The “visual monitoring scale” was created by the authors solely for this study (McIntosh et
    al., 2010) with no reported reliability or validity. The authors chose three questions from the
    Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales (CSBS) that LSAC had used to assess infants’
    communication skills and readiness to learn language (Wetherby & Prizant, 2001).
    The mother was asked how often her infant: looked at her to see if she was watching, tried
    to get her attention when she was being inattentive, and tried to get her to notice or look at
    interesting objects without trying to get her to do anything with them (p. 94). The
    researchers used these three questions to assess how watchful and wary the infants were in
    their mothers’ presence. Their rationale for choosing these three questions was that gazing
    frequently at the mother and trying to stay close to her were signs of insecurity and anxiety
    about the mother’s emotional availability according to some attachment theorists (p. 115).
    On the CSBS, however, these three behaviors indicate that the infant has more highly
    developed ways of communicating and is readier to begin talking. In other words, the
    frequent overnighters were the most advanced. In contrast, the preschooler researchers
    concluded that because the frequent overnighters gazed and tried to get their mothers’
    attention more often, they were significantly stressed, were having to work harder to
    monitor her presence and had “an added degree of vulnerability” (McIntosh et al., 2010, p.
    144).
    This interpretation is problematic on two counts. First, the three questions have never been
    established as valid or reliable measures of insecurity, anxiety, stress, or attachment. Second,
    there are reasons other than insecurity or stress why infants might engage in these three
    behaviors—one of which is being readier to learn to talk.
    The second measure was the mother’s yes or no answer to one question: ‘Does your child
    wheeze at night more than four times a week?’
    The LSAC researchers had used this question as part of a scale to assess health or sleep
    problems. The study’s authors, however, used this one question as a measure of children’s
    stress because they classified wheezing as a “psychosomatic” variable: “Higher rates of
    wheezing in the shared care group are congruent within the attachment/stress hypothesis”
    (McIntosh et al., 2010, p. 147).
    Further they assumed that the stress was caused by a “negative emotional environment” in
    the shared care families:
    “As outlined in the literature review, several studies confirm a link between a
    negative emotional family environment and onset of asthma and wheezing in
    infancy” (p. 147).
    These assumptions and interpretations are questionable for several reasons. To begin, using
    a single question is not a valid or reliable method for assessing any factor (Carmines &
    McIver, 1981).
    “With a single measure of each variable, one can remain blissfully unaware of
    the possibility of measurement error, but in no sense will this make his
    inferences more valid” (Blalock, 1970, p. 111).
    Also, classifying wheezing as a psychosomatic reaction to infant stress is unwarranted for at
    least three reasons (Carmo, 2009; NCHS, 2010; Reyes, 2011). First, parents’ reports are not
    reliable measures of children’s wheezing. Indeed, infant wheezing can be difficult even for
    physicians to detect.
    Second, wheezing in and of itself is not a validated measure for assessing stress because
    wheezing can be caused by environmental, genetic, and physio- logical factors having
    nothing to do with stress or family dynamics. Even the authors briefly acknowledged this
    fact:
    “Wheezing was independently predicted by low parental income” (McIntosh
    et al., 2010, p. 148).
    Third, the link between wheezing and family stress is not as simple or as straightforward as
    implied, as evidenced by the three studies cited by the authors to support their hypothesis –
    none of which confirmed a significant link between family stress and wheezing. In the first
    study cited (Berz, 2007) the factors predicting asthma or wheezing for the 2- to 3-year-olds
    were: gestational age, the mother’s having asthma, the child’s being male, adults’ smoking in
    the home, parents not having social support, parent having seen violence in the
    neighborhood or family, and high maternal anxiety.
    These researchers pointed out, however, that highly anxious mothers may be overly
    sensitive to infants’ breathing difficulties, meaning we cannot determine whether mothers’
    anxiety contributes to infants’ wheezing. In the second study cited (Shankardass, 2009) pre-
    schoolers were at greater risk for developing asthma if their mothers scored in the top
    quartile on a stress questionnaire. However, asthma was only more likely when the children
    were being exposed to traffic air pollution:
    “We observed little effect of stress in the absence of exposure to oxidant pollutants” (p.
    12410).
    More important still, this study actually addressed the question: Is living in two homes
    associated with more asthma or wheezing? The answer was no. Residing in two homes was
    not linked to the children’s asthma or wheezing and had less impact than: people’s smoking
    in the home, living in a trailer or an apartment, and having a pet, cockroaches, mildew, or
    bedroom carpet in the home. In the third study cited (Klinnert, Kaugars, Strand, & Silveira,
    2008) in which all of these 4-year-olds were at risk for developing asthma because they had
    been diagnosed by doctors with wheezing illnesses as infants, the authors concluded;
    “The contribution of the family stress composite was attenuated when other
    variables were taken into account” (p. 51).
    In sum, using mothers’ reports on one question about wheezing as an indication of infant
    stress, and then attributing that stress to a negative emotional environment in shared care
    families—a variable that was never assessed in the study – is unwarranted.
    The fourth and fifth measures, the irritability scale and the persistence scale, also had
    problems related to validity and interpretation. The irritability scale only had an alpha of .57
    (Sanson & Mission, 2005), which is considered “questionable” reliability (George & Mallery,
    2003). No reliability or validity was reported for the persistence scale (Sanson, Prior, &
    Garino, 1987).
    Moreover, the persistence and the irritability scores are difficult to interpret as “good” or
    “bad” because the scales provide no way to differentiate healthy/normal scores from
    unhealthy/abnormal ones.
    Equally important, although the researchers interpreted irritability and lower persistence as
    signs of the child’s inability to regulate emotions, other interpretations are equally plausible,
    among them: intestinal problems such as colic, attention deficit disorders, or the baby’s
    difficult temperament. In short, the irritability and persistence scales were not validated
    measures for assessing infant stress, or developmental problems, or emotional regulation
    difficulties.
    It is also worth noting that these measures were based entirely on the mothers’ reports,
    even though the researchers have reported elsewhere that these data came from
    independent observers’ reports of the babies’ general day to day behavior (McIntosh &
    Smyth, 2012, p. 178).
    Again, these shortcomings do not mean the study should be dismissed. All studies have
    shortcomings. Then too, these authors had no control over the lack of validity and reliability
    for the measures used by LSAC. The point is that the woozle overlooks this study’s
    limitations. As Warshak (2012) stated:
    “It is somewhat surprisingly that this heavily flawed study from Australia is
    being raised in serious debates about family law reform” (p. 12).
    Likewise, other scholars have concluded that the study made only a limited contribution to
    the debate on overnighting (Parkinson & Cashmore, 2011). Lamb, an internationally
    recognized expert on early childhood development, also concurred that the study was
    relatively insignificant given the statistical insignificance and ambiguity of most of the
    findings (Lamb, 2012b).
    To be more blunt, other scholars have stated that this study should never have been used to
    make policy recommendations cautioning against overnighting (Ludolph & Dale, 2012).
    [emphasis added – RW ]
    The Data Versus the Woozle
    One defining hallmark of a woozle is reporting and exaggerating some findings while ignoring
    others. The question, therefore, is how much of a discrepancy exists between the actual data
    from the study and the woozle: Overnighting even as little as once a week increases infants
    and toddlers’ problems in regard to: irritability, persistence at tasks, stress induced
    wheezing, behavioral and emotional regulation problems, and distressed behavior and
    insecure attachments to their mothers.
    First and foremost, there were no significant differences between overnighting and non-
    overnighting infants on four of the six measures of well-being: their mothers’ concerns about
    the infants’ development, overall physical health, wheezing, and negative responses to
    strangers.
    Second, there were no consistent relationships between overnighting and the outcomes on
    any of the measures other than persistence at tasks.
    Third, in regard to trying to get their mothers’ attention (which the researchers interpreted
    negatively as being watchful and wary), the infants who frequently overnighted were no
    different from infants who occasionally overnighted.
    Fourth, not only were frequent overnighters not more whiny and irritable than infants who
    never overnighted, they had exactly the same mean irritability score as infants living in intact
    families. Because these researchers were interpreting irritability as a sign of poor stress
    regulation related to insecure attachment, this would mean that most Australian infants
    from intact families also had insecure attachment and emotional/stress regulation problems.
    In short, there appears to be a “whining woozle” when the data are presented as if frequent
    overnighting contributes to abnormally high levels of irritability.
    Ignoring these findings, the woozle focuses solely on the three negative outcomes for the
    frequently overnighting infants – outcomes that largely failed to show consistent
    relationships.
    First, for irritability, the 43 frequent overnighters were more irritable than the 14 occasional
    overnighters— but not more than the 115 who never overnighted. Second, for wheezing
    (interpreted as a psychosomatic reaction to stress), the 38 frequent overnighters wheezed
    more (p.=.08, approaching but not achieving significance) than the 18 occasional
    overnighters— but again, not more than the 121 who never overnighted. Third, for gazing
    and trying to get their mothers’ attention (interpreted as signs of insecurity and anxiety), the
    59 frequent overnighters gazed and sought attention more often than the 141 infants who
    never overnighted – but again, not more than the 18 infants who occasionally overnighted.
    In short, there was no clear connection between overnighting and wheezing, irritability, and
    attention seeking – all of which were based on measures with no reported reliability or
    validity. Similar patterns emerged for the 2- and 3-year-olds. There were no differences
    between the frequent and the occasional overnighters on three of the seven measures:
    emotional functioning, conflict with caregivers, negative responses to stranger. On overall
    health, the frequent overnighters were healthier than the non-overnighters.
    Likewise, on wheezing (interpreted as a sign of stress) the frequent overnighters had better
    outcomes than both the occasional and the non-overnighters. Completely ignoring these
    positive data, the wheezing woozle focuses instead on the less significant negative finding (p.
    = .08) for the infants: the 38 frequent overnighters wheezed more often than the 18
    occasional overnighters.
    Moreover, the 2- to 3-year-olds did not have more problems interacting with their peers or
    in overall social adjustment (McIntosh et al., 2010, p. 137). Their difficult behavior was
    limited to their interactions with their mothers. The researchers interpreted this finding
    negatively:
    “Of concern, but as predicted by attachment theory, they also showed
    severely distressed behaviors in their relationship with the primary parent”
    (McIntosh et al., 2010, p. 9).
    In fact, however, these types of behaviors were relatively common in the general population
    of Australian children. In the LSAC survey from which the preschooler data were taken, 4,400
    mothers reported that 50% of their 2- to 3-year-olds cried, whined and hung onto her when
    she tried to leave, 50% sometimes refused to eat, and 40% often got very upset with her.
    According to the preschooler researchers, this would mean that half of all Australian toddlers
    were exhibiting “severely distressed behaviors” due to attachment distress with their
    mothers. Equally important, the frequent overnighters’ mean score (32.82) on the behavioral
    problems scale was well within the normal range (scores ” 36 ! high/abnormal; Smart, 2010),
    meaning they were not “severely distressed.”
    In other words, babies and toddlers who frequently overnighted were no more irritable and
    no more difficult with their mothers than infants and toddlers in married families. Given this,
    drawing negative conclusions about shared care parenting plans based on these two
    measures is unwarranted.
    It is true, however, that the 19 frequent overnighters had worse scores on the five item
    persistence scale than the toddlers who occasionally or never overnighted. This finding is
    worth noting as long as several limitations are kept in mind. First, this five item scale that
    was adapted from a longer standardized scale has no reported validity or reliability;
    therefore, it is not clear what is being measured. Second, there is no way to interpret these
    scores because the scale does not provide a way to differentiate healthy / normal scores
    from unhealthy/abnormal ones.
    In other words, there is no way of knowing whether the shared care children’s mean score (X
    = 3.93) indicated that they had any more significant or more noticeable problems than the
    toddlers with rare overnights (X = 4.24) or with occasional overnights (X = 4.13).
    Without knowing whether the shared care children’s mean was within a normal range, there
    is no basis for contending that shared care has a more negative impact than the other
    parenting plans. Despite these limitations, we will now see that the study ended up being
    presented and perceived as valid, reliable, and unequivocal evidence to support the woozle.
    Evidence by Citation: “Reviews” of the Research
    As previously explained, a woozle often begins when one or two studies are repeatedly cited
    as if they represented all, or almost all, of the research on a particular topic—and when
    those few studies are then used as the basis for policy recommendations. Shortly after the
    preschooler study was published in May, 2010, two articles (Fehlberg, Smyth, Maclean, &
    Roberts, 2011b; Trinder, 2010) purporting to be general reviews of the research on children
    of all ages in shared parenting families (referred to as shared care / shared residence in these
    articles) and one (Rathus, 2010) claiming to “examine the contemporary social science
    literature” (Rathus, 2010, p. 165) were published.
    It should also be noted that the second author of the Fehlberg et al. (2011b) paper was the
    second author of the preschooler study. The three articles made custody law
    recommendations for all children—not just for children under the age of 5 years. All three
    articles included the pre-schooler study, while none included more than five of the other 28
    empirical studies that had compared the outcomes for children in shared parenting families
    to children in sole physical custody families.
    In this way, the preschooler study was given more attention and disproportionate weight in
    regard to custody recommendations than the other 23 studies. Rather than basing their
    recommendations on the 28 studies that had compared the outcomes for the children in
    primary care and in shared parenting families, the authors cherry-picked only those few
    studies that supported their recommendation against shared parenting legislation.
    The first article (Fehlberg et al., 2011b) recommended that the British parliament not enact
    laws that would prioritize shared time over other parenting arrangements—a
    recommendation the authors reiterated in a policy paper (Fehlberg, Smyth, Maclean, &
    Roberts, 2011a). The section on young children (p. 8) reported only the negative data from
    the preschooler study, ignoring the fact that there were no differences on most measures,
    and never mentioning the better outcomes for the shared care children on several
    outcomes.
    The article also ignored Pruett’s (2004) study that found equal or better outcomes for infants
    and toddlers who overnighted and failed to mention that the Solomon and George study
    (1999) found no differences in attachments between the overnighting and the non-
    overnighting infants. The second article (Trinder, 2010) also advocated against shared
    parenting legislation in the United Kingdom.
    As with the Fehlberg article, the section on “younger children” (pp. 491–492) reported only
    the negative data from the preschooler study, entirely ignored Pruett’s study (Pruett et al.,
    2004) and mistakenly reported the Solomon and George (1999) study as having found higher
    rates of insecure attachment in
    infants who overnighted. Likewise the third article (Rathus, 2010) concluded that the 2006
    custody law reforms in Australia were ill-advised for two reasons.
    First “the reforms were driven by fathers’ rights groups” (p. 164). Second “shared time
    orders have created a ‘lego-science’ that shared parenting is almost always good for
    children, but this lego-science is a pseudoscience which is not consistent with the complex
    reported social science about shared parenting” (p. 164).
    In sum, within a short period of time the hallmarks of a woozle were emerging: putting
    considerable emphasis on only one or two studies to the exclusion of the others and making
    policy recommendations on the basis of only that portion of the available data that could be
    used to advocate for a particular position – in this case, to advocate against shared parenting
    legislation for children of all ages and to advocate against overnighting for infants and
    toddlers.
    Misrepresentations of Similar Studies
    Another situation that contributes to the creation of a woozle is claiming that several studies
    reached the same conclusion, when in fact they did not. This makes it appear as if the woozle
    is based on a wider base of empirical evidence than is actually the case.
    In that regard, the Solomon and George study (1999) and the preschooler study are often
    cited together as having reached similar conclusions about infant overnighting (McIntosh,
    2011c; McIntosh, 2011f; McIntosh, 2012a; McIntosh, 2012c; McIntosh, Burke, Dour, &
    Gridley, 2009). As previously explained, however, Solomon and George (1999) concluded
    that there were no significant differences in attachment classifications between the
    overnighters and non-overnighters, regardless of frequency of overnighting.
    Moreover, Solomon and George concluded that the overnighters’ having more disorganized
    (unclassifiable) attachments than the infants in married families (but not more disorganized
    than infants who did not overnight) was due to the negative characteristics of their parents’
    relationship, not to the overnighting.
    Woozle Fertilizers: From Academia to Media
    The Media
    One of the richest fertilizers for a woozle is repeated andwidespread exposure over an
    extended period of time – especially in the media and especially when the topic is a
    controversial one.
    The pre-schooler study illustrates this pattern. In the years before the study was released in
    2010, Australians were debating whether to reform their custody laws in ways that would be
    more supportive
    of shared parenting (referred to as “shared care” in Australia when children live with their
    fathers more than 35% of the time).
    In 2006 these controversial reforms were enacted, but the debates continued. This is
    important because, as already noted, woozles are more likely to arise when the public has
    strong feelings about the controversial issues underlying them.
    The lead author of the preschooler study was often quoted – correctly or incorrectly—as
    saying that research showed that shared care for preschoolers and overnighting for infants
    and toddlers had a deleterious impact on children. It was not clear what research McIntosh
    was referring to in some of her older interviews.
    For example, in “Trouble Ahead for Babies of Divorce” the article began:
    “The majority of babies who live alternately with their divorced parents
    develop long-lasting psychological problems, new research has found. Such
    arrangements cause enduring disorganised attachment in 60% of infants
    under 18 months, says clinical psychologist and family therapist, Jennifer
    McIntosh” (Martin, 2003, p. 1).
    In later interviews, however, McIntosh was specifically referring to the preschooler study
    (Horin, 2010; “Infants Struggle in Shared Care,” 2010). Although some reporters may not
    have accurately represented what was said to them, there was nonetheless a consistency in
    what they reported (Biggs, 2009; Clinton, 2008; Kissane, 2007).
    More recently, after interviewing McIntosh about the preschooler study, the interviewer
    wrote that shared care was a “developmental disaster” and that attachment studies “from
    around the world” showed babies cannot cope with a change of their primary caregiver
    without suffering physical and psychological problems (Jackman, 2010). Illustrating how
    grossly distorted data can become, one reporter wrote that there were indications of
    “violent behavior” in the shared care toddlers (Diwan, 2010).
    In and of themselves, media reports cannot create a woozle. But in combination with other
    factors, the media can prime the public to accept woozles.
    Academic Journals and Conferences
    Along with the media exposure, the study received considerable attention in academic
    journals and at conferences. The largest organization for family court professionals, the
    Association of Family and Conciliatory Courts (AFCC), put McIntosh in charge of editing a
    special issue on attachment, which included recommendations on parenting plans for infants
    and toddlers (McIntosh, 2011a).
    Many of the statements in her introductory summary reinforced the conclusions of the
    preschooler study and the particular version of attachment theory on which it relied. Among
    these conclusions were:
    “Overnight care is not essential to an infant or child’s ability to form a healthy
    attachment to the second parent.”
    “All contributors agreed on the essential role of a “primary” attachment figure
    in the first year or two of life.”
    “In normal development, the female brain is specifically equipped for the
    largely non-verbal, affiliative, nurturant aspects of attachment formation with
    an infant.”
    “Overnight stays away from the primary caregiver in early infancy are
    generally best avoided, unless of benefit to the primary caregiver (McIntosh,
    2011a, p. 423).
    McIntosh (2011a) reassured readers that she had presented a balanced and thorough
    overview of the current research:
    “Anyone in the know about attachment will agree: this is a stellar,
    comprehensive line-up of experts” (p. 421).
    Many scholars, however, did not agree with the theoretical perspectives or the conclusions
    in the special edition and also expressed their concerns about the preschooler study itself
    (Garber, 2012; Hynan, 2012; Lamb, 2012a; Ludolph, 2012). Their primary criticisms were that
    McIntosh chose to include only those researchers who agreed with her points of view and
    who thereby endorsed the preschooler study, that current research on attachment had been
    ignored, and that the recommendations against overnighting far over reached the empirical
    data.
    In response, McIntosh replied,
    “Although some may want to continue to shoot me as the messenger, I stand
    by this special issue for the answers it offers at this point in time, while
    recognizing that some questions it raises may ultimately prove more
    important” (McIntosh, 2012c, p. 500).
    Likewise, in response to another critique (Parkinson & Cashmore, 2011), neither of the lead
    authors of the pre-schooler study acknowledged the validity of any of the criticisms (Smyth,
    McIntosh, & Kelaher, 2011).
    Receiving further attention, the preschooler study was the focus of the plenary address that
    McIntosh and George (coauthor of the study with Judith Solomon) presented at the
    Association of Family and Conciliation Courts (AFCC) national conference in 2012, which was
    attended by more than 1,000 people (McIntosh, 2012b).
    In contrast to the earlier summaries of the study with Solomon, George now stated that their
    study reached the same conclusion as the preschooler study: Overnighting interferes with
    infants’ attachments to their mothers. McIntosh then mistakenly stated that Pruett’s (Pruett
    et al., 2004) study had reached similar conclusions to their two studies:
    “To cut a long story short, we took these findings, looked at the other studies,
    saw a pattern” (p. 5).
    As previously explained, however, Pruett did not find significant differences between the
    overnighting and the non-overnighting two to three year-olds (Pruett et al., 2004). Further,
    McIntosh stated:
    “There have been attempts throughout the field to polarize us and our
    studies” (p. 3). “We have found the purpose, designs and findings of our
    respective studies twisted beyond recognition, and motivations and intent
    attributed to us that defie belief ” (p. 4).
    Given her concerns about the woozling of their data, it is unfortunate that McIntosh
    inadvertently may have misled the audience to believe that thousands of overnighting
    children had been in the preschooler study, when in fact there were as few as 14 children in
    some of the groups:
    “We explored a large randomly selected general population dataset. This
    amounts to 10,000 children 0–5 years” (p. 4).
    Throughout 2011 and 2012 the study’s findings were further disseminated through
    international seminars. These included the lead author’s presentation to the New Zealand
    Psychological Society (McIntosh, 2010a), her video on shared parenting for the Minnesota
    Bar Association, an interview for their newsletter
    (Jeske, 2011; Waggoner, 2011), a seminar hosted by the Guardian Ad Litem Association in
    Massachusetts (McIntosh, 2011d), and a seminar in London hosted by Liz Trinder who had
    written the “research review” article advocating against shared parenting custody reform in
    the United Kingdom. The seminar was sponsored by the Nuffield Foundation, which had
    published reports and co-authored letters to the Prime Minister opposing custody reform
    (CYPFD, 2012).
    The brochure stated that the seminar was “based on the highest quality research evidence
    available internationally.” “McIntosh is widely acknowledged as the leading international
    expert on the effects on children of shared care” (McIntosh, 2011e, p. 1). There is certainly
    nothing unprofessional about researchers’ presenting their data to audiences around the
    world.
    Disseminating data is commendable as long as the data are not being presented in a way
    that gives an unbalanced view of the literature or that advocates for a particular position
    that is not supported by the literature. But in regard to what the woozle was claiming about
    overnighting and about shared parenting, the problem was that the other six studies that
    had included children under the age of 5 years (two of which focused exclusively on children
    as young as the children in the preschooler study) were not receiving this type of public and
    international attention.
    The Woozle’s Emotional Hook
    As explained earlier, presenting case studies, hypothetical situations, and anecdotal stories
    that the audience might misperceive as being representative of the general body of research
    can contribute to a woozle.
    For example, in a 2-day seminar in Australia with McIntosh, George (2012) offered a
    hypothetical example of a child in shared care. The child, Frankie, was being taken care of
    every week by a cadre of adults: both parents, four grandparents, two sets of
    stepparents/partners, the staff in two different day care centers (because the parents could
    not agree on a day care center) and the tot care staff at church (p. 13).
    By providing such an atypical and negative example, George might have inadvertently led her
    audience to believe that this was the typical situation for most preschoolers in shared care
    families. Moreover, this hypothetical example directly contradicted the empirical data from
    Pruett’s (Pruett et al., 2004) study: Having multiple caretakers was not related to negative
    outcomes for infants or toddlers – and was, in fact, related to even better outcomes for girls.
    Similarly, McIntosh wrote an article for the AFCC newsletter that reaches thousands of family
    court professionals – a story about a teenage mother (that McIntosh had met by accident in
    a train station) who
    was ordered by the court to have her 1-year-old baby live on alternate weeks with its father
    (McIntosh, 2010b). Although such anecdotal stories and case studies are compelling, Emery
    (2005) is among those social scientists who caution:
    “We all have to recognize and admit that clinical experience, including case
    studies, prove nothing” (pp. 9–10).
    And as explained earlier, anecdotal stories and dramatic case studies can trigger emotional
    response that threaten to override critical thinking and empirical data.
    The Impact of the Woozle
    Through many pathways over recent years, the pre-schooler study has morphed into the
    woozle: infant or toddler overnighting and shared parenting for preschoolers have a
    deleterious impact on infants and other children under the age of 4 years.
    But is there any evidence that this particular woozle has had any impact on public policies or
    public opinion? The answer appears to be yes. In Australia the report containing the pre-
    schooler study and a second study with older children was delivered to Attorney General
    Robert McClelland in May, 2010. Six months later he cited the report as part of the “strong
    evidence base” for his proposed amendment to revoke the 2006 shared parenting laws
    (Jackman, 2010).
    Just weeks before the national elections, Fehlberg, whose review of the research article had
    largely excluded all studies except the preschooler study, also spoke out against the 2006
    custody law reforms in Sydney’s Morning Herald (Fehlberg, 2010). In the same year, the
    study was also presented at the national conference of the Australian Family Law Association
    (McIntosh, 2010a).
    The study also had an impact on three influential organizations in Australia: the Australian
    Psychological Society, the Association for Infant Mental Health, and the National Council for
    Children Post Separation (2013). All three recommended or warned against overnighting for
    infants and shared care for other children under the age of four, citing only two empirical
    studies: the pre-schooler study and the study by Solomon and George (1999).
    McIntosh was the lead author of the infant overnight care paper (McIntosh, 2011c) which
    was the background paper for the AAIMH guidelines (AAIMH, 2011) and was lead author of
    the position statement paper for the Australian Psychological Society (McIntosh et al., 2009).
    Many of the statements in these documents were similar to statements that McIntosh made
    one year later in the special issue of Family Court Review—statements that other scholars
    criticized for misrepresenting and overreaching the research, as previously discussed
    (McIntosh, 2011a). The Infant Mental Health guidelines were disseminated by the Australian
    media (Griffin, 2011; Overington, 2011), as well as by law firms’ web sites that warned
    against overnighting and shared care (Magee, 2010; O’Loughlin, 2011).
    In the United States the study has also had an impact – in some instances, an impact limited
    to recommendations against over-nighting for infants and toddlers, but, in others, extending
    to custody recommendations for children of all ages. Among the articles citing the pre-
    schooler study as the basis for warning against overnighting and shared parenting for
    children under the age of four were: an article in the Huffington Post by a professor of
    human development (Hughes, 2011), another in the Minnesota Bar Association’s newsletter
    (Jeske, 2011), and another in the Wisconsin Journal of Family Law (Zirkel, 2012).
    An article in the Maryland Bar Association Journal cited only two studies in expressing its
    opposition to legal presumptions of shared parenting for children of all ages—one being the
    pre-schooler study (Fait, Wills, & Borenstein, 2012). The “no overnighting” message has also
    been posted on at least one national parenting web site (Markham, 2013).
    The study also has had an impact in some states on proposed shared parenting legislation, as
    well as on overnight parenting plans for the very youngest children. In Oregon the legislative
    advisory committee’s report on custody reform recommended against considering a shared
    parenting bill. The “Summary of current information and research” report included only four
    of the 28 available empirical studies on outcomes for children in shared parenting families –
    one of which was the preschooler study (Scher & Vien, 2011).
    Likewise, the Minnesota Matrimonial Lawyers Association, after having watched a taped
    presentation by McIntosh at their conference, concluded that overnighting for infants and
    toddlers was ill advised (“Splitting the Baby,” 2011).
    The taped presentation was also discussed by members of the Minnesota Family Law
    Association (Waggoner, 2011). More recently in Alabama, a children’s advocacy organization
    presented a series of public seminars throughout the state, explaining their legislative
    priorities for 2014—one of which is to oppose the proposed shared parenting legislation.
    One of the handouts states:
    “Neuroscience shows that consistency is critical especially for young children (0 –3) and the
    developing brain. Moving from place to place, even when there are two loving and fit
    parents, is not good for young children” (Voices, 2013, p. 6).
    The one source cited as the basis for the handout was the article about the pre-schooler
    study, “Infants Struggle in Shared Care” (2010), posted on the web site of an Australian
    university where the lead author is an adjunct teacher.
    Moving beyond the United States, in Israel during the time the government was holding
    legislative meetings about reforming custody laws, McIntosh was the keynote speaker at a
    video conference sponsored by female leaders at Bar Llan University’s gender studies
    program (Whiston, 2012). What McIntosh actually said in her speech and whether the video
    was ever used to try to persuade legislative committees to vote against shared parenting
    custody laws is known only to the people involved. But regardless of its intent or its content,
    the presentation was interpreted as supporting the woozle: Infants and toddlers should
    spend little, if any, overnight time with their fathers and women, unlike men, have brains
    that are “hard wired” to form the primary attachment and to communicate with infants.
    The ire aroused by the speech was evident in headlines such as this on the Internet:
    “Australian male bashing guru Jennifer McIntosh calls Israel to avoid shared parenting”
    (“Australian Male Bashing,” 2012). Many scholars and policymakers were concerned enough
    about the possible impact of the seemingly one-sided presentation of current research that
    they invited Warshak (2012) to present a balanced overview of the literature relevant to
    parenting plans for very young children.
    Likewise, in the United Kingdom the study has played a role in current debates over custody
    reform. The committee that was assigned to make custody reform recommendations to the
    British Parliament (Norgrove, 2011) cited only three of the 28 studies that had compared
    outcomes for shared parenting children and for other children with separated parents.The
    pre-schooler study was one of the three (Rhoades, 2011).[emphasis added – RW ]
    The committee recommended that Parliament not consider shared parenting legislation:
    “Drawing on international and other evidence we oppose legislation to encourage shared
    parenting. The detailed information from Australia showed the damaging consequences for
    many children” (p. 138). Trinder, who was well acquainted with the pre-schooler study
    because she had hosted McIntosh’s 2011 seminar in London, (McIntosh, 2011e), also
    provided a consultation response to the committee, stating her approval of their decision
    and claiming it was consistent with the research (Norgrove, 2011, p. 138).
    [See also Men’s Aid rebuttal of Trinder’s somewhat dated citations, circa 1980s. (Ref CF100
    http://www.publications.parliament.uk/pa/cm201213/cmpublic/childrenandfamilies/memo
    /cf100.pdf) – RW]
    Even more recently in a letter to the prime minister, the directors of eight counseling and
    advocacy centers voiced their opposition to shared parenting legislation, all citing the
    preschooler study without citing any of the other 28 studies (CYPFD, 2012). This is not to say
    the preschooler study was the pivotal factor in these policy recommendations.
    But because it was cited in all of these documents to the exclusion of almost all of the other
    studies that have examined outcomes for children in shared parenting families, it serves as
    yet another example of the extent to which the preschooler study was being presented
    internationally to policymakers.
    In sum, as is characteristic of woozles, the findings from the preschooler study seem to have
    grown larger and to have become more significant with the passage of time, while its
    limitations seem to have all but disappeared from view.
    How to Corral a Woozle: Damage Control
    Once a woozle is on the loose, how can it be corralled? One approach is for researchers to
    point out the limitations of the data on which the woozle is predicated. As already noted, a
    number of researchers have written about the limitations of the preschooler study. All of
    these critiques, however, have been published in academic journals—meaning they are
    unlikely to attract any public attention, let alone the attention of the media.
    Given this, a more effective approach might be to involve the media in disseminating the
    research that contradicts the woozle. Likewise, researchers could share more of the research
    with those organizations and legislative committees whose reports or policy
    recommendations have been based on woozled data.
    Researchers whose studies are being used to support a woozle can also take steps to limit or
    to repair the damage. By persistently and publicly correcting the misunderstandings of their
    data, they can counter some of the misleading reports in the media and in academic and
    professional settings. Likewise, they can respond to critiques of their work by de-
    personalizing the debates and welcoming academic disagreements, never attempting to
    interfere in any way with anyone’s candid expressions of their views.
    In the same vein, willingly sharing seminar and conference materials reduces the odds of
    being misperceived as having contributed to the woozling of one’s own data. Authors can
    also refrain from
    using case studies and personal anecdotes or from presenting their data in overly dramatic
    ways that can easily be misused by advocacy groups to promote a particular position.
    Finally researchers should exercise caution when reporting their data when their findings are
    not consistent with the existing body of research, or when there are still very few studies on
    the topic.
    Authors of studies that have already been woozled—or studies that stand a good chance of
    becoming woozled because of their controversial findings—also need to be consistent and
    unambiguous when presenting their study’s findings and when using their own data to
    support their own positions on matters of policy.
    Authors should ensure that their synopses, abstracts, summaries, speeches, and seminar
    materials consistently correspond to their study’s full report and to the full analyses of their
    data. Likewise all of their published work and recommendations should be consistent with
    what they say in their seminars and at conferences.
    By sending conflicting or ambiguous messages to different audiences, researchers are
    equivocating in ways that may inadvertently fortify woozles based on their data. In contrast,
    by presenting the same summaries and making the same recommendations to all audiences,
    researchers reduce the chances of having their data distorted into woozles.
    Being clear and consistent also protects researchers from being perceived as being
    disingenuous or as intentionally woozling their own data. Likewise, researchers can protect
    their data from being woozled by realizing that whether they couch their comments as
    “advice,” “recommendations,” “guidelines,” “contraindications,” or “rules,” they are likely to
    be perceived in the same way by the general public, practitioners and policymakers – even if
    experts and scholars are able to make these distinctions in nomenclature.
    In closing, several points are worth repeating. First, the particular study presented in this
    article is only one of many studies that could be used to illustrate the process that leads to a
    woozle. I chose this particular study to illustrate how woozles are created and the impact
    they can have because this is an area where I am familiar with the research—and because
    the topic is currently in the forefront of worldwide debates on custody law reform, infant
    overnighting and shared parenting plans for the very youngest children. Second, no single
    person or no one event can be held responsible for the creation or the promotion of a
    woozle. A constellation of factors, including the media and advocacy groups, carry the
    woozle along its path. Finally, in regard to custody law reform and parenting plans, we need
    to ensure that all of the available data are widely disseminated to the public, policymakers,
    and practitioners.
    To do otherwise is to do a grave disservice to the millions of children whose parents are no
    longer living together. We want to be sure that, unlike Winnie the Pooh, we do not base our
    decisions or our opinions on a woozle.
    References:-
    AIFS. (2012). Longitudinal study of Australian children: Growing up Australia. Melbourne:
    Australian Institute of Family Studies. Available at http://www.growingupinaustralia.gov.au/
    Andreassen, C., & Fletcher, P. (2007). Early childhood longitudinal study: Psychometric report
    for the 2 year data collection. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics.
    doi:10.1037/e661962007-001
    Australian Association of Infant Mental Health. (2011). Infants and overnight care: post
    separation and divorce guidelines for protecting the very young child’s sense of comfort and
    security. Double Bay, Australia
    Australian male bashing guru Jennifer McIntosh calls on Israel to avoid shared parenting.
    (2012, Jan. 8). Free Press Release.
    Berz, J. B. (2007). Prevalence and correlates of early onset asthma and wheezing in 2 to 3
    year olds. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 154–166. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsj123
    Best, J. (2001). Damned lies and statistics: Untangling numbers from the media, politicians
    and activists. Berkeley: University of California.
    Best, J. (2008). Stat spotting: A field guide to identifying dubious data. Los Angeles: University
    of California.
    Best, J. (2013). More damned lies and statistics. Berkeley: University of California.
    Biggs, B. (2009, Spring). Shared parenting hurting children. Australian Family Magazine, 17.
    Blalock, H. (1970). An introduction to social research. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
    Carmines, E., & McIver, J. (1981). Analyzing models with unobserved variables. In G.
    Borhnstedt & E. Borgatta (Eds.), Social measurement: Current issues (pp. 65–115). Beverly
    Hills, CA: Sage.
    Carmo, B. (2009). Minor psychiatric disorders in mothers and asthma in children. Social
    Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 44, 416–420. doi:10.1007/s00127-008-0450-x
    Cashmore, J., & Parkinson, P. (2011). Parenting arrangements for young children: Messages
    for research. Australian Journal of Family Law, 25, 236–257.
    Chabris, C., & Simons, D. (2010). The invisible gorilla: And other ways our intuitions deceive
    us. New York, NY: Crown.
    Clinton, M. (2008, November 9). Children suffer when law splits parenting equally. Courier
    Mail Brisbane,
    Cope, M., & Allison, D. (2010). White hat bias: Examples of its presence in obesity research
    and a call for renewed commitment to faithfulness in research reporting. International
    Journal of Obesity, 34, 84–88. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2009.239
    Center for Research on Child Wellbeing. (2013). Parental incarceration and child wellbeing in
    fragile families. Princeton, NJ: Author.
    CYPFD. (2012). Response to proposed shared parenting legislation.
    Cheshire, England. Cooperative Parenting Following Divorce Association.
    Diwan, P. (2010, July 8). Kids may be left in trauma by shared parenting. Available at
    http://www.topnews.inc
    Emery, R. (2005). Parental alienation syndrome: Proponents bear the burden of proof. Family
    Court Review, 43, 8–13. doi:10.1111/j.1744-1617.2005.00002.x
    Fait, D., Wills, V., & Borenstein, S. (2012). The merits and problems with presumptions for
    joint custody. Maryland Bar Journal, 14, 13–17.
    Fehlberg, B. (2010, August 26). Shared care laws damaging many children [Opinion piece].
    The Sydney Morning Herald.
    Fehlberg, B., Smyth, B., Maclean, M., & Roberts, C. (2011a). Caring for children after parental
    separation: Would legislation for shared parenting time help children? Oxford, Eng.:
    University of Oxford, Department of Social Policy and Intervention.
    Fehlberg, B., Smyth, B., Maclean, M., & Roberts, C. (2011b). Legislating for shared time
    parenting: A research review. International Journal of Law, Policy and the Family, 25, 318–
    337. doi:10.1093/lawfam/ebr015
    Garber, B. (2012). Attachment and family law: Commentary on the July 2011 issue. Family
    Court Review, 55, 457–470.
    Gelles, R. (1980). Violence in the family: A review of research in the seventies. Journal of
    Marriage and the Family, 42, 873–885. doi: 10.2307/351830
    Gelles, R. (2007). The politics of research: The use, abuse and misuse of social science data.
    Family Court Review, 45, 42–51. doi:10.1111/j.1744-1617.2007.00127.x
    George, C. (2012, August). Divorce in the nursery. Paper presented at the Family Transitions
    Counseling Center, Sydney, Australia.
    George, D., & Mallery, P. (2003). SPSS for windows. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
    Griffin, M. (2011, December 15). Shared custody a mistake for the under 2s. Sydney Morning
    Herald.
    Horin, A. (2010, July 8). Shared babies at risk of anxiety. Sydney Morning Herald.
    Houghton, B. (1979, November). Review of research on women and abuse. Paper presented
    at the American Society of Criminology National Conference, Philadelphia, PA.
    Hughes, R. (2011, July 21). New research insights into guidance about parenting plans.
    Huffington Post.
    Hynan, D. (2012). Young children, attachment security and parenting schedules. Family Court
    Review, 55, 471–480.
    Infants struggle in shared care. (2010, July 7). La Trobe University News, p. 1.
    Jackman, C. (2010, November 24). Divided lives. The Australian Magazine, p. 1.
    Jeske, J. (2011, June 7). Issues in joint custody and shared parenting: Lessons from Australia.
    Minnesota Bench and Bar Newsletter, p. 1.
    Johnston, J. (2007). Introducing perspectives in family law and social science research. Family
    Court Review, 45, 15–21. doi:10.1111/j.1744-1617.2007.00125.x
    Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.
    Kaspiew, R., Gray, M., Weston, R., Moloney, L., Hand, K., & Qu, L. (2009). Evaluation of 2006
    family law reforms in Australia. Sydney: Australian Institute of Family Studies.
    Kissane, K. (2007, December 21). Custody rules are exposing children to war zones. Available
    at http://www.TheAgeMagazine.com
    Kline, M., Tschann, J., Johnston, J., & Wallerstein, J. (1989). Children’s adjustment in joint and
    sole physical custody families. Developmental Psychology, 25, 430–438. doi:10.1037/0012-
    1649.25.3.430
    Klinnert, M. D., Kaugars, A., Strand, M., & Silveira, L. (2008). Family psychological factors in
    relation to children’s asthma status and behavioral adjustment at age 4. Family Process, 47,
    41–61. doi:10.1111/j.1545-5300.2008.00238.x
    Lamb, M. (2012a). Critical analysis of research on parenting plans and children’s well-being.
    In K. Kuehnle & L. Drozd (Eds.), Parenting plan evaluations: Applied research for the family
    court (pp. 214–246). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
    Lamb, M. (2012b). A wasted opportunity to engage with the literature on attachment
    research. Family Court Review, 54, 481–485.
    Lamb, M., & Kelly, J. (2001). Using the empirical literature to guide the development of
    parenting plans for young children. Family Court Review, 39, 365–371. doi:10.1111/j.174-
    1617.2001.tb00618.x
    Lilienfeld, S. O. (2012). Public skepticism of psychology. American Psychologist, 67, 111–129.
    doi:10.1037/a0023963
    Ludolph, P. (2012). Special issue on attachment: Overreaching theory and data. Family Court
    Review, 50, 486–495. doi:10.1111/j.1744-1617.2012 .01464.x
    Ludolph, P., & Dale, M. (2012). Attachment in child custody: An additive factor, not a
    determinative one. Family Law Quarterly, 46, 225–245.
    Maccoby, E., & Mnookin, R. (1992). Dividing the child. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
    Press.
    Magee, P. (2010). Shared care vs. the child’s best interest. Sydney, Australia. Available at
    http://www.ArmstrongLegalFamilyLaw.org
    Markham, L. (2013). Divorce: Protecting your kids. Available at www .AhaParenting.org
    Martin, L. (2003, October 21). Trouble ahead for babies of divorce. Sydney Morning Herald.
    McClanahan, S. (2011). Outcomes for children in fragile families. In P. England & M. Carlson
    (Eds.), Changing families in an unequal society (pp. 108–134). Palo Alto, CA: Stanford
    University Press.
    McIntosh, J. (2010a). Shared parenting after separation. Canberra: Australian National Family
    Law Conference.
    McIntosh, J. (2010b, Spring). We miss flights for a reason. AFCC Newsletter, p. 3.
    McIntosh, J. (2011a). Guest editor’s introduction to the special issue: Attachment theory,
    separation and divorce. Family Court Review, 49, 418–425. doi:10.1111/j.1744-
    1617.2011.01382.x
    McIntosh, J. (2011b). Infants and overnight care: Guidelines for protecting the very young
    child’s sense of comfort and security: Guideline 1. 1–3 Sydney: Australian Association for
    Infant Mental Health.
    McIntosh, J. and the Australian Assocation for Infant Mental Health. (2011c). Infants and
    overnight care post separation and divorce: Clinical and research perspectives. Background
    paper for the Australian Association for Infant Mental Health, Sydney.
    McIntosh, J. (2011d, October). Shared parenting: Solomon’s dilemma in the 21st century.
    Paper presented at the seminar sponsored by Regis College and Massachusetts Guardian Ad
    Litem Association, Weston,
    McIntosh, J. (2011e, May). Shared parenting: What works for children? Paper presented at
    the seminar on the Network on Family, Regulation and Society, London, England.
    McIntosh, J. (2011f). Special considerations for infants and toddlers in separation/divorce:
    Development issues in the family law context. In R. Tremblay, M. Boivin, & R. Peters (Eds.),
    Encyclopedia on early childhood development. Montreal, Canada: Center of Excellence for
    Early Childhood Development. Available at http://www.child-encyclopedia
    .com/pages/PDF/McIntoshANGxp1.pdf
    McIntosh, J. (2012a, August). Divorce in the nursery: Achieving attachment security for
    infants in a family law context. Seminar: Family Transitions Mediation Center, Sydney,
    Australia.
    McIntosh, J. (2012b, June). Infants and overnights: The drama, the players and their scripts.
    Paper presented at the Association of Family and Conciliation Courts, Chicago, IL.
    McIntosh, J. (2012c). Reviewing the opportunities: Reply to comments. Family Court Review,
    50, 496 –501. doi:10.1111/j.1744-1617.2012 .01465.x
    McIntosh, J., Burke, S., Dour, N., & Gridley, H. (2009). Parenting after separation: A position
    statement prepared for the Australian Psychological Society. Sydney, Australia.
    McIntosh, J., Smyth, B., Kelaher, M., & Wells, Y. L. C. (2010). Post separation parenting
    arrangements: Outcomes for infants and children. Sydney: Australian Attorney General’s
    Office.
    McIntosh, J., & Smyth, B. (2012). Shared time parenting: An evidence based matrix for
    evaluating risk. In K. Kuehnle & L. Drozd (Eds.), Parenting plan evaluations: Applied research
    for the family court (pp.
    155–188). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
    Melli, M., & Brown, P. (2008). Exploring a new family form—The shared time family.
    International Journal of Law, Policy and Family, 22, 231–269. doi:10.1093/lawfam/ebn002
    Milar, P., & Kruk, E. (in press). Maternal attachment, paternal overnight contact: Response to
    Tornello, et al. Journal of Marriage and Family, 44.
    Milne, A. A. (1926). Winnie the Pooh. London, England: Metheun.
    NCHS. (2010). Childhood asthma: Diagnosis by family structure 2009.
    Washington, DC: National Center for Health Statistics.
    National Council for Children Post Separation. (2013). Shared parenting: The myth that it is in
    the best interest of children. Sydney, Australia:
    Nielsen, L. (2013a). Shared residential custody: A recent research review. American Journal
    of Family Law, 27, 61–72.
    Nielsen, L. (2013b). Shared residential custody. American Journal of Family Law, 27, 123–137.
    Norgrove, D. (2011). Family justice review final report. London, England: Ministry of Justice.
    O’Loughlin, A. (2011, April). Developmental outcomes for young children in shared parenting
    arrangements. Retrieved from http://www.CoulterRoucheLawFirm.Newsletter.com
    Overington, C. (2011, December). Shared custody: New thoughts on what’s best for kids.
    http://www.Mama Mia magazine for Australian women.
    Park, R. (2000). Voodoo science: The road from foolishness to fraud. New York, NY:
    Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1063/1.1325202
    Parkinson, P., & Cashmore, J. (2011). Parenting arrangements for young children – A reply to
    Smyth, McIntosh and Kelaher. Australian Journal of Family Law, 25, 284–286.
    Peterson, R. (1996a). A re-evaluation of the economic consequences of divorce. American
    Sociological Review, 61, 528–536. doi:10.2307/2096363
    Peterson, R. (1996b). Statistical errors, faulty conclusions, misguided policy: Rely to
    Weitzman. American Sociological Review, 61, 539–540. doi:10.2307/2096365
    Pruett, M., Cowan, P., Cowan, M., & Diamond, J. (2012). Supporting father involvement after
    separation and divorce. In K. Kuehnle & L. Drozd (Eds.), Parenting plan evaluations: Applied
    research for the family court (pp. 257–330). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
    Pruett, M., Ebling, R., & Insabella, G. (2004). Critical aspects of parenting plans for young
    children. Family Court Review, 42, 39–59.
    Pudasainee-Kapri, S., & Razza, R. (2013). Attachment security among toddlers (Working
    Paper). Washington, DC: Fragile Families Research Institute.
    Rathus, Z. (2010). Social science or lego science? Queensland University of Technology Law
    and Justice Journal, 2, 164–190.
    Reyes, M. (2011). Relationship between maternal demoralization, wheeze and
    immunoglobulin in inner city children. Annals of Allergy, Asthma and Immunology, 107, 42–
    49.
    Rhoades, H. (2011). Report in relation to shared parenting. The Norgrove Committee Report
    to British Parliament (pp. 215–224). London, England: Ministry of Justice.
    Rosik, C. H., Jones, S., & Byrd, D. (2012). Knowing what we do not know. American
    Psychologist, 67, 498–499. doi:10.1037/a0029683
    Sanson, A., & Mission, S. (2005). Summarising children’s wellbeing: LSAC outcome index.
    Sydney: Australian Institute of Family Studies.
    Sanson, A., Prior, M., & Garino, E. (1987). The structure of infant temperament: Factor
    analysis of the revised infant temperament questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development,
    10, 97–104. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(87)90009-9
    Scher, L., & Vien, E. (2011). Custody and parenting time: Summary of current information
    and research. Portland, OR: State Family Law Committee.
    Schore, A., & McIntosh, J. (2011). Family law and the neuroscience of attachment. Family
    Court Review, 49, 501–512. doi:10.1111/j.1744-1617.2011.01387.x
    Shankardass, K. (2009). Parental stress increases the effect of traffic related air pollution on
    childhood asthma incidence. Proceedings of National Academy of Science, 106, 12406–
    12411. doi:10.1073/ pnas.0812910106
    Smart, D. (2010). How children are faring: Behaviour problems and competencies. Sydney:
    Australian Institute of Family Studies.
    Smyth, B., McIntosh, J., & Kelaher, M. (2011). Research into parenting arrangements for
    young children: Comment on Cashmore and Parkinson. Australian Journal of Family Law, 25,
    258–271.
    Solomon, J. (2013). An attachment theory framework for planning infant and toddler
    visitation. In L. Gunsberg & P. Hymowitz (Eds.), Handbook of divorce and custody (pp. 259–
    278). New York, NY: Routledge.
    Solomon, J., & George, C. (1999). The effects on attachment of overnight visitation on
    divorced and separated families: A longitudinal follow up. In J. Solomon & C. George (Eds.),
    Attachment disorganization in atypical populations (pp. 243–264). New York, NY: Guilford
    Press.
    Sommers, C. (1994). Who stole feminism? New York, NY: Simon & Schuster. Splitting the
    baby: The psychology and economics of shared parenting. (2011, September). Paper
    presented at the conference of the Minnesota Chapter of the American Association of
    Matrimonial Lawyers, Breezy Point, MN.
    Tornello, S., Emery, R., Rowen, J., Potter, D., Ocker, B., & Xu, Y. (2013). Overnight custody
    arrangements, attachment and adjustment among very young children. Journal of Marriage
    and Family, 75, 871–885.
    Trinder, L. (2010). Shared residence: A review of recent research evidence. Child and Family
    Law Quarterly, 22, 475–498.
    van IJzendoorn, M. H., Vereijken, C., Kranenburg, M., & Walraven, M. (2004). Assessing
    attachment security with the attachment Q sort: Meta-analytic evidence for the validity of
    the observer AQS. Child Development, 75, 1188–1213. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
    8624.2004.00733.x
    Voices. (2013, October). Time to get moving: 2014 legislative policy priorities. Birmingham:
    Voices for Alabama’s Children.
    Waggoner, P. (2011). Discussion of Jennifer McIntosh’s video on shared parenting.
    Minneapolis: Minnesota Family Law Meeting.
    Warshak, R. (2002). Who will be there when I cry in the night? Revisitng overnights: A
    rejoinder to Biringen, et al. Family Court Review, 40, 208–219. doi:10.1111/j.174-
    1617.2002.tb00832.x
    Warshak, R. (2012, May). Securing children’s best interests while resisting the lure of simple
    solutions. Paper presented at the conference of Parenting in Practice and Law, University of
    Haifa, Israel.
    Warshak, R. (in press). Social science and parenting plans for young children. Psychology,
    Public Policy and Law.
    Waters, E. (2013). Assessing secure base behavior and attachment security using the Q-sort
    method. Stony Brook: State University of New York.
    Waters, E., & Deane, K. (1985). Defining and assessing individual differences in attachment
    relationships. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development, 50, 41–65.
    doi:10.2307/3333826
    Weisberg, D. S. (2008). The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. Journal of
    Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 470–477. doi:10.1162/jocn .2008.20040
    Weitzman, L. (1985). The divorce revolution. Boston, MA: Free Press.
    Weitzman, L. (1996). The economic consequences of divorce are still unequal: Comment on
    Peterson. American Sociological Review, 61, 537–538. doi:10.2307/2096364
    Wetherby, A., & Prizant, B. (2001). Communication and symbolic behavior scales
    developmental profile. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes Publishing Company.
    Whiston, R. (2012). Israel gets McIntoshed. London, England: Dads for kids Fatherhood
    Foundation.
    Zirkel, K. (2012). Custody and placement of infants and toddlers: Towards a synthesis of
    recent research. Wisconsin Journal of Family Law, 32, 1–4.